2019
DOI: 10.1101/801126
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neurodevelopmental shifts in learned value transfer on cognitive control during adolescence

Abstract: Value-associated cues in the environment often enhance subsequent goal-directed behaviors in adults, a phenomenon supported by integration of motivational and cognitive neural systems.Given the interactions among these systems change throughout adolescence, we tested when beneficial effects of value associations on subsequent cognitive control performance emerge during adolescence. Participants (N=81) aged 13-20 completed a reinforcement learning task with four cue-incentive pairings that could yield high gain… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
1
1

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Eighty-nine participants ages 8- to 25-years-old (M age = 16.16, SD age = 4.67, 45 female) were included in analyses. A target sample size of n = 90, including 30 children, 30 adolescents, and 30 adults, was determined based on prior work using similar or smaller sample sizes to identify age-related differences in behavior and brain activation (Van Den Bos and Rodriguez, 2015; Insel et al, 2019; Callaghan et al, 2021). Data exclusions consisted of: eight participants with excessive motion (participants without at least one complete encoding, baseline arrows, and post-encoding arrows runs due to exclusions of runs with 15% or more timepoints censored with greater than 0.9 mm relative translational motion), seven participants who elected to not complete or terminate the fMRI scan, and five participants with incomplete datasets as a result of fMRI scanner malfunction.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Eighty-nine participants ages 8- to 25-years-old (M age = 16.16, SD age = 4.67, 45 female) were included in analyses. A target sample size of n = 90, including 30 children, 30 adolescents, and 30 adults, was determined based on prior work using similar or smaller sample sizes to identify age-related differences in behavior and brain activation (Van Den Bos and Rodriguez, 2015; Insel et al, 2019; Callaghan et al, 2021). Data exclusions consisted of: eight participants with excessive motion (participants without at least one complete encoding, baseline arrows, and post-encoding arrows runs due to exclusions of runs with 15% or more timepoints censored with greater than 0.9 mm relative translational motion), seven participants who elected to not complete or terminate the fMRI scan, and five participants with incomplete datasets as a result of fMRI scanner malfunction.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Eighty-nine participants ages 8-to 25-years-old (Mage = 16.16, SDage = 4.67, 45 female) were included in analyses. A target sample size of n = 90, including 30 children, 30 adolescents, and 30 adults, was determined based on prior work using similar or smaller sample sizes to identify age-related differences in behavior and brain activation (Van Den Bos and Rodriguez, 2015;Insel et al, 2019;Callaghan et al, 2021). Data exclusions consisted of: eight participants with excessive motion (participants without at least one complete encoding, baseline arrows, and postencoding arrows runs due to exclusions of runs with 15% or more timepoints censored with greater than 0.9 mm relative translational motion), seven participants who elected to not complete or terminate the fMRI scan, and five participants with incomplete datasets as a result of fMRI scanner malfunction.…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%