2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103107
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

No evidence for an independent retinotopic reference frame for inhibition of return

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Change detection also breaks the assumption that the search array will remain stable across multiple fixations leading to a state in which inhibitory tags are not maintained. IOR as a facilitator in search is possible because it is typically coded in scene/environmental coordinates (Malevich et al, 2020;Maylor & Hockey, 1985;Posner & Cohen, 1984), and scene removal has been shown to eliminate IOR (Klein & MacInnes, 1999;Müller & Mühlenen, 2000;Takeda & Yagi, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Change detection also breaks the assumption that the search array will remain stable across multiple fixations leading to a state in which inhibitory tags are not maintained. IOR as a facilitator in search is possible because it is typically coded in scene/environmental coordinates (Malevich et al, 2020;Maylor & Hockey, 1985;Posner & Cohen, 1984), and scene removal has been shown to eliminate IOR (Klein & MacInnes, 1999;Müller & Mühlenen, 2000;Takeda & Yagi, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…IOR may serve an additional purpose in visual search by inhibiting previously attended locations and reducing the likelihood of refixation at previously attended locations (Klein, 1988;Posner & Cohen, 1984). It can be encoded in scene coordinates (Malevich et al, 2020;Maylor & Hockey, 1985), it is long lasting (Samuel & Kat, 2003), and it is observed in serial search (Klein, 1988). IOR was thus chosen as the key mechanism for preventing refixations in many computational models of salience (Itti & Koch, 2000;Krasovskaya & MacInnes, 2019 for an overview).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This task was carried out to examine whether the IOR effect at the remapped retinal locus, if any, was an artifact caused by the spillover of IOR at the cued location (He et al, 2015;Malevich et al, 2020). The display setup was the same as that in the return-saccade task, except that no saccade was required before the onset of the probe.…”
Section: No-remapping Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This observation contradicts the fact that IOR is closely linked to the oculomotor system (for a recent review of the neural basis of IOR, see Satel et al, 2019). Recent studies have revealed robust IOR effects in both eye-centered and world-centered coordinates immediately following saccades (Hilchey et al, 2014;Krüger & Hunt, 2013;Mathôt & Theeuwes, 2010;Pertzov et al, 2010;Yan et al, 2016; but see Malevich et al, 2020; see Fig. 1c-d for a graphical review), indicating that IOR is natively coded in eyecentered brain maps but (predictively) remaps to maintain at spatially relevant locations.…”
mentioning
confidence: 92%
“…While robust evidence suggests the coordinate system of attentional facilitation primarily resides in retinotopic coordinates, maintaining visual stability in spatiotopic coordinates is still crucial, and it is possible that other types of attentional mechanisms may operate in spatiotopic coordinates. For example, previous studies showed evidence for spatiotopic inhibition of return (IOR; Hilchey et al, 2012;Malevich et al, 2020;Maylor & Hockey, 1985;Pertzov et al, 2010;Posner & Cohen, 1984) which is an attentional bias against recently attended locations (Posner, 1980). However, findings on the coordinate system of IOR are less clear-cut than the retinotopic facilitation effects described above (Abrams & Pratt, 2000;Krüger & Hunt, 2013;Mathôt & Theeuwes, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%