2003
DOI: 10.1086/367579
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

No Time To Eat: An Adaptationist Account Of Periovulatory Behavioral Changes

Abstract: A comprehensive review of women's dietary behavior across the menstrual cycle suggests a drop in caloric intake around the time of ovulation; similar patterns occur in many other mammals. The periovulatory nadir is puzzling, as it is not explicable in terms of changes in the energy budget. Existing explanations in the animal literature operate wholly at the proximate level of analysis and hence do not address this puzzle. In this paper, I offer an ultimate explanation for the periovulatory feeding nadir, argui… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
45
1
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 77 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 108 publications
(122 reference statements)
5
45
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Given that all behaviors necessarily entail opportunity costs, we can therefore expect that, if they are governed by adaptations, motivations to engage in behaviors that are not tied directly to conception will exhibit a periovulatory reduction. For example, in many mammals, including humans, caloric intake is reduced around ovulation, thereby reducing foraging time in a manner that frees up time for mate-seeking and mating (Fessler, 2003). By the same token, independent of conscious intentions as regards reproduction, we might expect women to prioritize opposite-sex sexual contact over same-sex sexual contact during the fertile window.…”
Section: Progesteronementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Given that all behaviors necessarily entail opportunity costs, we can therefore expect that, if they are governed by adaptations, motivations to engage in behaviors that are not tied directly to conception will exhibit a periovulatory reduction. For example, in many mammals, including humans, caloric intake is reduced around ovulation, thereby reducing foraging time in a manner that frees up time for mate-seeking and mating (Fessler, 2003). By the same token, independent of conscious intentions as regards reproduction, we might expect women to prioritize opposite-sex sexual contact over same-sex sexual contact during the fertile window.…”
Section: Progesteronementioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is possible that had we used higher doses or a more prolonged treatment with raclopride, we would have observed a similar reversal too. Shortlatency and short-lasting phase bursts of activation of ventral tegmental area DA projections to the hippocampus (El-Ghundi et al, 1999, 2003Li et al, 2003;Moncada and Viola, 2007) are believed to foster learning by highlighting novel aspects of a stimulus and/or a response that is contingently or subsequently paired with a reward (Schultz, 2006). In this view, our findings on the STFP for a novel food item are in agreement with results showing that SCH23390 could block novelty seeking in rats (Peters et al, 2007) and the impaired reversal learning of D1KO mice in both spatial and food rewarded learning (El-Ghundi et al, 1999, 2003.…”
Section: Effects Of the Dopaminergic D1-type And D2-type Antagonists mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Researchers have used as few as two to as many as 6 phases to examine the menstrual cycle (Cohen et al, 1987;Fessler, 2003) resulting in a lack of uniformity, consistency and duration. In this study we remedied the problem by only examining women with documented surges in luteinizing hormone (LH) to establish strictly defined menstrual cycle phases.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%