2012
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-012-0292-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Nonprobative photographs (or words) inflate truthiness

Abstract: When people evaluate claims, they often rely on what comedian Stephen Colbert calls "truthiness," or subjective feelings of truth. In four experiments, we examined the impact of nonprobative information on truthiness. In Experiments 1A and 1B, people saw familiar and unfamiliar celebrity names and, for each, quickly responded "true" or "false" to the (betweensubjects) claim "This famous person is alive" or "This famous person is dead." Within subjects, some of the names appeared with a photo of the celebrity e… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

14
129
5
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 139 publications
(149 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
14
129
5
1
Order By: Relevance
“…When people evaluate general knowledge claims (e.g., Macadamia nuts are in the same evolutionary family as peaches) that they know are a mix of true and false, they more often endorse claims that appear with related, but nonprobative, photos (e.g., a photo of macadamia nuts; Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012). But these findings cannot tell us whether photos similarly distort people's judgments about their personal pasts.…”
mentioning
confidence: 58%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…When people evaluate general knowledge claims (e.g., Macadamia nuts are in the same evolutionary family as peaches) that they know are a mix of true and false, they more often endorse claims that appear with related, but nonprobative, photos (e.g., a photo of macadamia nuts; Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012). But these findings cannot tell us whether photos similarly distort people's judgments about their personal pasts.…”
mentioning
confidence: 58%
“…We used unfamiliar animals for two reasons. First, the effects of nonprobative photos tend to be more powerful for judgments about unfamiliar stimuli (Newman et al, 2012). Second, we wanted to make it difficult for people to encode the events well because the test phase happened soon after the study phase, and there is evidence that well-encoded experiences are less susceptible to the influence of nondiagnostic feelings than are poorly -encoded experiences (Monin, 2003;Zaragoza & Lane, 1998).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Manipulations like these that boost comprehension can also make other concepts related to the material feel more easily available in memory, and we know that people interpret this feeling of ease as diagnostic of familiarity and truth (Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012;Tversky & Kahneman, 1973;Whittlesea, 1993;see Alter & Oppenheimer, 2009, for a review). But a brain image depicting activity in the frontal lobes is different.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We also know from recent work that nonprobative photos can more rapidly nudge people to find associated claims true (Fenn, Newman, Pezdek, & Garry, 2013;Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012). In this paradigm, people quickly judge a series of trivia claims (The liquid metal inside a thermometer is magnesium) as true or false.…”
Section: Photographs Promote Ease Of Imaginationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…
Abstract When people rapidly judge the truth of claims about the present or the past, a related but nonprobative photo can produce Btruthiness,^an increase in the perceived truth of those claims (Newman, Garry, Bernstein, Kantner, & Lindsay, 2012). What we do not know is the extent to which nonprobative photos cause truthiness for the future.
…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%