2000
DOI: 10.1080/03637750009376500
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Norms, expectations, and deception: A norm violation model of veracity judgments

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
47
1
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(55 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
6
47
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…considered a suspect by investigators, found guilty, sentenced more severely). This proposition is in line with the normative violation model of Levine et al (2000); in the theory of Levine et al ''behavior violating norms [i.e. socially inappropriate behavior] will be judged as less honest than norm-consistent behavior'' (p. 127).…”
Section: The Role Of Expectations For Emotionsupporting
confidence: 70%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…considered a suspect by investigators, found guilty, sentenced more severely). This proposition is in line with the normative violation model of Levine et al (2000); in the theory of Levine et al ''behavior violating norms [i.e. socially inappropriate behavior] will be judged as less honest than norm-consistent behavior'' (p. 127).…”
Section: The Role Of Expectations For Emotionsupporting
confidence: 70%
“…There was incriminating evidence present during the investigation and presented during the trial, but if Pam Smart's lack of emotion created suspicion in the investigators and/or jurors, that suspicion would seem to be well placed given the trial's outcome. Levine et al (2000) explain that, while expectations can come from norms (''situationally based standards for behavior,'' p. 124), they can also come from other sources. For example, prior interaction with the target person or information from an informed source can lead one to expect normal or abnormal behavior.…”
Section: The Influence Of Emotion In Actual Cases: Some Considerationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Farquhar, 2005). Research shows that people are more truth-biased when interacting face-to-face (Buller et al, 1991), when they know the person they are talking to (McCornack & Parks, 1986), and they are not forewarned of impending deception (e.g., Levine et al, 2000;McCornack & Levine, 1990). Each of these conditions is often met in everyday situations, but is seldom the case in deception detection experiments.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Explanations for truth-bias include the cognitive processes involved in mentally representing true and false information (Gilbert, 1991) and fundamental principles in how language is understood (Grice, 1989;McCornack, 1992). Truth-bias is especially pronounced when people interact face-to-face (Buller, Strzyzewski, & Hunsaker, 1991), when people know the message source (McCornack & Parks, 1986), and in studies where the participants are unaware that their task is to detect lies (e.g., Levine et al, 2000;McCornack & Levine, 1990). Levine et al (1999) observed that the research documenting the accuracy-is-slightlyabove-50% conclusion typically share two important features.…”
Section: Deception Detection Accuracymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous theoretical work focusing on violations of social norms (e.g., Levine, et al, 2000) and expectations for nonverbal behavior (Burgoon & LePoire, 1993) suggests that, when norm violations are perceived negatively, people who violate norms are judged less favorably than people who follow norms. Although minor interruptions and back channel cues are expected, or even preferred during informal conversations, institutionally constituted talk, such as televised political debates, usually have formally constructed rules for who can speak, how long each debater can speak, the appropriate topics for discussion, and so on (e.g., Tracy, 2002).…”
Section: Review Of Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%