2015
DOI: 10.1017/s0954394515000101
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Not always variable: Probing the vernacular grammar

Abstract: Written and spoken language are known to differ substantially (Biber, 1988; 1995; Biber, Johansson, Leech, Conrad, & Finegan, 1999). Standard written language is highly uniform and governed by prescription, whereas the vernacular is most revealing of structured heterogeneity (Weinreich, Labov, & Herzog, 1968). We focus on four English morphosyntactic variables that problematize assumptions about the nature of variation in the vernacular: the genitive, the comparative, the dative, and relative pronouns.… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
13
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 83 publications
2
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Rather than one form obsolescing, the end point of this grammatical change may be longitudinal partitioning (see e.g. D'Arcy & Tagliamonte 2015). While Labov (1982: 76) emphasized the need for in-depth linguistic analysis in order to uncover the ‘ordered series of shifts in underlying probabilities associated with each environmental factor’, he possibly did not foresee that different constraints within the same variable might exhibit distinct trajectories.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rather than one form obsolescing, the end point of this grammatical change may be longitudinal partitioning (see e.g. D'Arcy & Tagliamonte 2015). While Labov (1982: 76) emphasized the need for in-depth linguistic analysis in order to uncover the ‘ordered series of shifts in underlying probabilities associated with each environmental factor’, he possibly did not foresee that different constraints within the same variable might exhibit distinct trajectories.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In our data, direct objects longer than six words categorically favor the continuous variant ( n = 947 of 953, 99.4%), while the continuous order is almost never used when the direct object is a personal pronoun ( n = 7 of 3245, 0.2%). Thus, tokens containing pronominal direct objects or direct objects longer than 6 words were excluded from the analysis to restrict the scope of the study to truly variable contexts (D'Arcy & Tagliamonte, 2015). The final dataset consisted of n = 11,340 PV tokens.…”
Section: Data Sources Variable Extraction and Annotationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Who as a relativizer is restricted to human head nouns (D'Arcy and Tagliamonte : 392), which necessitates limiting the envelope of variation to [+human] head nouns on top of subject gaps. Since the Ø and which in (9) and (10) are both very rare in this context in the TEA (D'Arcy and Tagliamonte : 389–392, : 272), the variation in human subject relativizers is almost entirely represented by that and who . In the TEA, these variants exhibit a classic age‐graded pattern, with who as the standard form (D'Arcy and Tagliamonte : 393).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Relativization corresponding to subject gaps (D'Arcy and Tagliamonte , : 266–267, 271–272) involves four variants in most dialects of present‐day English, shown in (8) through (11).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%