2001
DOI: 10.1017/s0142716401004076
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Noun phrase morphology in Swedish-speaking children with specific language impairment

Abstract: Children with specific language impairment (SLI) are often described as having great difficulty with grammatical morphology, but most studies have focused only on these children's use of verb morphology. In this study, we examined the use of noun phrase (NP) morphology by preschool-age children with SLI who are acquiring Swedish. Relative to typically developing same-age peers and younger peers matched according to mean length of utterance, the children with SLI had greater difficulty in the use of genitive in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

4
35
1

Year Published

2002
2002
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
4
35
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The second research question was whether the children with SLI selected are representative of children with this diagnosis. Based on the results presented here, together with the results from the same children presented in other studies (6,7,18), we suggest that they are: already at the outset the children were ensured to follow the exclusionary criteria and to follow the inclusionary criteria as well (the results from the LuMat). Closer scrutiny of the results from the assessments does not contradict this.…”
Section: Phonological Processessupporting
confidence: 71%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The second research question was whether the children with SLI selected are representative of children with this diagnosis. Based on the results presented here, together with the results from the same children presented in other studies (6,7,18), we suggest that they are: already at the outset the children were ensured to follow the exclusionary criteria and to follow the inclusionary criteria as well (the results from the LuMat). Closer scrutiny of the results from the assessments does not contradict this.…”
Section: Phonological Processessupporting
confidence: 71%
“…This means that although the language matching was based on MLU, the two groups performed at comparative levels on both general grammatical production (the LuMat) and on language comprehension (the SIT). This is interesting, given the ndings that the children with SLI lag behind the MLU controls on several grammatical structures, for example the use of copula and past tense forms (6,7) and in the use of genitive ¼s, inde nite articles and gender marking in noun phrases (18). From a clinical point of view, however, the difference between the children with SLI and the age-matched controls is the most relevant.…”
Section: Phonological Processesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Plural formation has been shown to be poorer in children at risk of dyslexia (e.g., Elbro, Borstrøm, & Petersen, 1998). It has also been shown to be delayed in children with SLI when compared to their age-matched peers (e.g., Conti-Ramsden, 2003;Leonard et al, 1997; or language-matched peers (e.g., Leonard, Bortolini, Caselli, McGregor, & Sabbadini, 1992;Leonard et al, 1997;Windsor, Scott, & Street, 2000) but it is relatively intact compared to other areas of morphological inflection (Bishop, 1994;Clahsen, Rothweiler, & Woest, 1992;Leonard, Salameh, & Hansson, 2001;Marshall, 2004;. Based on these findings, at-risk and SLI groups are expected to have more difficulties with plural marking than a control group.…”
Section: De Bree and Kerkhoffmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In French and other languages, one problem is that late talkers, who will eventually resolve their developmental delay, are often hard to distinguish from children who truly have a persistent linguistic impairment (Bates & Goodman, 1997; Paul, 1991; Thal, Tobias & Morrison, 1991; see also Desmarais, 2007, Demarais, Sylvestre, Meyer, Bairati & Rouleau, 2008, and Ellis & Thal, 2008, for systematic literature reviews on late talkers, mainly learning English). In addition, studies of French language development in pre-school children (Elin Thordardottir & Namazi, 2007; Le Normand, Leonard & McGregor, 1993; Royle & Elin Thordardottir, 2008) reveal few differences between normal and impaired language development on spontaneous-speech measures that have previously been useful in distinguishing children with SLI from normally developing children in other languages, such as English, Spanish, or Swedish (Leonard, Salameh & Hansson, 2001; Restrepro, 1998; Rice & Wexler, 1996). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…producing * la agua for el agua ‘det.m/f water’ ‘the water’), or omitting them (Restrepro & Gutierrez-Clellen, 2001). Swedish-speaking children aged 4 ; 3 to 5 ; 7 show difficulties with genitive - s and plural - er marking on nouns (all errors were omissions), determiner omissions, and gender substitutions with determiners and adjectives (neuter for uter gender or vice versa) in the noun phrase (Leonard et al , 2001). Finally, a subgroup of Italian-speaking children aged 4 ; 2 to 10 ; 7 show high levels of determiner omission in obligatory contexts (Bottari, Cipriani, Chilosi & Pfanner, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%