2004
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.01.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Number bias for the discrimination of large visual sets in infancy

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
119
2
1

Year Published

2005
2005
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 181 publications
(132 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
10
119
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Support for our view comes from studies that reveal that 5-year-old children already have an understanding of the number symbols that is comparable to that of adults (Huntley-Fenner 2001;Temple and Posner 1998) and they can, as well as infants can, perform basic mathematical procedures with nonsymbolic stimuli, e.g., arrays of dots (Barth et al 2003(Barth et al , 2006(Barth et al , 2005Lipton and Spelke 2005;Brannon 2002;Brannon et al 2004;Feigenson 2005;Jordan and Brannon 2006;Mix et al 2002;Xu and Spelke 2000;Mix et al 2002Mix et al , 2005. Note however, that Rousselle et al (2004) disentangled the effect of each continuous variable (e.g., density, area, contour length) on numerosity judgment performance in 3-year-old children and revealed that they performed at chance level in the condition that controlled for surface area.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 74%
“…Support for our view comes from studies that reveal that 5-year-old children already have an understanding of the number symbols that is comparable to that of adults (Huntley-Fenner 2001;Temple and Posner 1998) and they can, as well as infants can, perform basic mathematical procedures with nonsymbolic stimuli, e.g., arrays of dots (Barth et al 2003(Barth et al , 2006(Barth et al , 2005Lipton and Spelke 2005;Brannon 2002;Brannon et al 2004;Feigenson 2005;Jordan and Brannon 2006;Mix et al 2002;Xu and Spelke 2000;Mix et al 2002Mix et al , 2005. Note however, that Rousselle et al (2004) disentangled the effect of each continuous variable (e.g., density, area, contour length) on numerosity judgment performance in 3-year-old children and revealed that they performed at chance level in the condition that controlled for surface area.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 74%
“…Rodríguez and Gamboa 2000;Beran 2010;Krusche et al 2010;Rodríguez and Gloudeman 2011), and one of the challenges in research on numerical competence is to minimize the influence of confounding continuous variables (Lipton and Spelke 2003;Brannon et al 2004;Hurewitz et al 2006;Beran et al 2008). Communal predation on oecobiids occurs only among Portia juveniles within a specific size range (Jackson et al 2008), enabling us to test for the influence of a continuous variable (Portia stuff) on spider settling decisions by varying the size of already settled Portia over this range.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On average, six-month-old infants can discriminate ratios of 1:2 or easier, whereas most nine-montholds can discriminate the more difficult ratio of 2:3 (Brannon, Abbott, & Lutz, 2004;Lipton & Spelke, 2003Xu, 2003;Xu & Arriaga, 2007;Xu & Spelke, 2000;Xu, Spelke, & Goddard, 2005). Accuracy improves as children get older.…”
Section: Early Number Knowledge In Dual-language Learnersmentioning
confidence: 99%