1976
DOI: 10.2466/pms.1976.42.2.671
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Numerosity Discrimination of Tactile Stimuli

Abstract: The ability of 40 college students to discriminate the number of tactile stimuli presented simultaneously was measured in two experiments. Simulation was provided by 12 solenoids fixed to points on the arms and legs. Exp. I showed a mean correct discrimination level of between 1.55 and 2.10 solenoids. Exp II compared discrimination of numerosity by a group who received immediate feedback with a no-feedback control group. Feedback produced a small increase in this ability. However, the most stimulation points c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

1991
1991
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The inverse relationship between electrode position identification scores and number of stimulus electrodes is consistent with previously reported studies of absolute stimulus identification (Geldard and Sherrick, 1965;Gilson, 1968;Posey and James, 1976). In addition, it is of interest that the information transmission score for the single-electrode set was approximately 3 bits, consistent with values previously reported by Miller (1956) This lends support to the contention of a spreading interaction postulated in the masking studies.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…The inverse relationship between electrode position identification scores and number of stimulus electrodes is consistent with previously reported studies of absolute stimulus identification (Geldard and Sherrick, 1965;Gilson, 1968;Posey and James, 1976). In addition, it is of interest that the information transmission score for the single-electrode set was approximately 3 bits, consistent with values previously reported by Miller (1956) This lends support to the contention of a spreading interaction postulated in the masking studies.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…First, we provide a more nuanced perspective on the notion of numerosity, typically characterized as "a property of a stimulus that is defined by the number of discriminable elements it contains" (Brannon and Terrace 1998, 746). Numerosity traditionally refers to the magnitude of a collection of perceptual elements (e.g., "••••••" is more numerous than "•••"; Ginsburg 1976;Krishna and Raghubir 1997;Lechelt 1975;Palat et al 2014;Pelham et al 1994;Piazza and Izard 2009;Piazza et al 2007;Posey and James 1976;Strauss and Curtis 1981), but marketing scholars also have started to adopt this label to describe the magnitude of a symbolic number (6 is more numerous than 3; Lembregts and Pandelaere 2013;Monga and Bagchi 2012;Wertenbroch and Soman 2007). Any quantity can be expressed in smaller versus larger symbolic numbers, independent of whether it is discretized in fewer versus more elements, so our studies highlight the importance of distinguishing perceptual numerosity (i.e., number of elements in an array) from symbolic numerosity (i.e., magnitude of an Arabic numeral).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The order of presentation of the two block types was randomized across participants. Note that the number of tactors activated in any pattern never exceeded three, given recent evidence that people's ability to detect simultaneously presented stimuli over the body surface shows a marked decrease if the number of tactors activated exceeds this number (Gallace et al, 2006b; see also Posey & James, 1976).…”
Section: Experiments 1 Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%