2018
DOI: 10.1111/bjso.12272
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Obedience without orders: Expanding social psychology's conception of ‘obedience’

Abstract: Psychologists have typically defined obedience as a form of social influence elicited in response to direct orders from an authority figure. In the most influential set of studies of obedience, conducted by Stanley Milgram in the early 1960s, the orders at the disposal of the authority figure were a series of verbal prods. However, recent research has suggested that Milgram's experiments do not show people following orders. It has therefore been suggested that the experiments are not demonstrations of obedienc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
22
0
2

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
(101 reference statements)
0
22
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Similar arguments have been made with respect to actors' disavowals of responsibility in arguing that they were "just following orders" (Gibson, 2018). For example, in a rhetorical analysis of post-experiment interviews with participants from the original Milgram studies, Gibson et al (2018) argued that participants' displacement of responsibility onto the experimenter was essentially motivated reasoning-post-hoc, self-serving justifications for their behavior in the study.…”
Section: Motivated Reasoningmentioning
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similar arguments have been made with respect to actors' disavowals of responsibility in arguing that they were "just following orders" (Gibson, 2018). For example, in a rhetorical analysis of post-experiment interviews with participants from the original Milgram studies, Gibson et al (2018) argued that participants' displacement of responsibility onto the experimenter was essentially motivated reasoning-post-hoc, self-serving justifications for their behavior in the study.…”
Section: Motivated Reasoningmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…As noted by Galinsky (), “When you're in charge, your whisper may feel like a shout.” Nonetheless, projecting their own deafness onto others, powerful people may raise their voices more and more because they fail to realize how loud they already sound to those around them (cf., Gibson, ).…”
Section: Why Does This Distinction In Powerful People's Psychologicalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The issue of whether this means that the experiments are not demonstrations of obedience , however, depends on the conflation of obedience with following orders/commands. The standard social psychological definition of obedience certainly requires orders/commands for something to be understood in terms of obedience, but the key question is arguably whether this was ever a particularly good definition of obedience (Gibson, in press). In everyday language we might, for example, speak of obeying the law, but we do not need direct orders to do so.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…73-76). In contrast, contemporary researchers study in their own rights the dynamics of argumentation between the experimenter and the teacher, or repertoires of discursive/argumentative characteristics offered by (dis)obedient teachers (Gibson, 2013a(Gibson, , 2013b(Gibson, , 2014(Gibson, , 2017(Gibson, , 2019a(Gibson, , 2019bHollander, 2015;Hollander & Maynard, 2016;Hollander & Turowetz, 2017).…”
Section: Description Of Conditionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…434–443; cf., Gibson, , p. 298). Acts of ‘obedience’ thus appear not as submissions to orders but defeats in broadly understood argumentative battles (Gibson, , ; Hollander & Maynard, ).…”
Section: Departures and Continuity: The ‘Second Wave’ Of Criticismmentioning
confidence: 99%