1995
DOI: 10.1080/13506289508401729
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Object-centred orienting of attention

Abstract: In the present study two experiments are reported in which the subjects were presented on a computer screen with a two-dimensional line drawing that is perceived as a three-dimensionalobject (i.e. a cube). The cube could be seen as stationary, as rotating about the y-axis (Experiment 1A), or as rotating about the x-axis (Experiment 1B). The subject's attention was directed by a visual precue to a vertex of the cube. As the cube rotated, the precued location moved in viewer-centred co-ordinatesm, but the local … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

5
25
0

Year Published

1997
1997
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 51 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
5
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In conclusion, we note that the question of sharing versus switching is also of interest with respect to the general division between capacity theories and spotlight theories of spatial attention (e.g., Shaw, 1978;Umilta, 1988). The present evidence of sharing is certainly compatible with capacity models (cf.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 73%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In conclusion, we note that the question of sharing versus switching is also of interest with respect to the general division between capacity theories and spotlight theories of spatial attention (e.g., Shaw, 1978;Umilta, 1988). The present evidence of sharing is certainly compatible with capacity models (cf.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 73%
“…It is also compatible with spotlight models that allow multiple locations to receive attention simultaneously (e.g., models including a diffuse attentional state or a gradient of attention), but it is incompatible with more restrictive spotlight models that allow only one attended location per trial (cf. Umilta, 1988). extract a maximum switching rate from studies of attentional movement.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Evidence that selective attention can operate within object-centred frames of reference has come from studies with both neuropsychologically impaired (e.g., Behrmann & Tipper, 1994, 1999Humphreys & Riddoch, 1994Tipper & Behrmann, 1996) and healthy (e.g., Gibson & Egeth, 1994;ReuterLorenz, Drain, & Hardy-Morais, 1996;Tipper et al, 1999;Umiltà et al, 1995) adults. In the spatial cueing literature, both facilitatory (e.g., Umiltà et al, 1995) and IOR (e.g., Gibson & Egeth, 1994;Tipper et al, 1999) cueing effects have been observed within object-centred frames of reference. These studies have shown that the two cueing effects can be associated with part of an object as it moves through space, as opposed to being tied to a fixed environmental location relative to the viewer.…”
Section: Object-based Cueing Effects In Moving Object Displaysmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These studies have shown that within-object locations can be coded in object-centred frames of reference (Fig. 2c), defined relative to one of the object's axes (e.g., elongation or symmetry), and that object-associated cueing effects remain unchanged, irrespective of changes in other, viewer-centred frames of reference (e.g., Gibson & Egeth, 1994;Reppa et al, 2010;Umiltà, Castiello, Fontana, & Vestri, 1995). For instance, Umiltà et al showed that facilitatory cueing effects were tied to the cued location within an object, irrespective of whether the cued location had changed relative to the viewer.…”
Section: Space-based Effects Of Spatial Cueingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation