2013
DOI: 10.1177/0956797613490746
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Object Shape and Orientation Do Not Routinely Influence Performance During Language Processing

Abstract: The role of visual representations during language processing remains unclear: They could be activated as a necessary part of the comprehension process, or they could be less crucial and influence performance in a task-dependent manner. In the present experiments, participants read sentences about an object. The sentences implied that the object had a specific shape or orientation. They then either named a picture of that object (Experiments 1 and 3) or decided whether the object had been mentioned in the sent… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

4
36
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
4
36
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Similar phenomena have been observed in the context of visual motion processing (e.g., Pirog et al, 2008; Dils and Boroditsky, 2010; but see Pavan and Baggio, 2010), color processing (e.g., Simmons et al, 2007), visual form processing (e.g. Zwaan et al, 2002; but see Rommers, Meyer, and Huettig 2013), gustatory processing (Simmons et al, 2013), and auditory processing (Kiefer et al, 2008)—and similar considerations apply as to whether the phenomena reflect the format of conceptual representations or rather the processing dynamics that obtain between amodal symbolic representations and input/output systems. What this means, concretely, is that we need explicit bridging hypotheses about the dynamics of activation flow in the system, and those bridging assumptions would need to be investigated prior to deriving inferences about representational format.…”
Section: Some Major Themes From This Volume and Their Broader Implicasupporting
confidence: 71%
“…Similar phenomena have been observed in the context of visual motion processing (e.g., Pirog et al, 2008; Dils and Boroditsky, 2010; but see Pavan and Baggio, 2010), color processing (e.g., Simmons et al, 2007), visual form processing (e.g. Zwaan et al, 2002; but see Rommers, Meyer, and Huettig 2013), gustatory processing (Simmons et al, 2013), and auditory processing (Kiefer et al, 2008)—and similar considerations apply as to whether the phenomena reflect the format of conceptual representations or rather the processing dynamics that obtain between amodal symbolic representations and input/output systems. What this means, concretely, is that we need explicit bridging hypotheses about the dynamics of activation flow in the system, and those bridging assumptions would need to be investigated prior to deriving inferences about representational format.…”
Section: Some Major Themes From This Volume and Their Broader Implicasupporting
confidence: 71%
“…The observation that object names enabled the long-term retrieval of object-color information stored in memory complements work on language comprehension showing that visual and motor representations of objects can be activated during word and sentence processing (for a review, see Zwaan, 2004; although see also Rommers et al, 2013). Such findings have often been interpreted in terms of sensorimotor theories of semantic memory whereby object knowledge is represented in a modality-specific rather than amodal fashion (Barsalou, 1999; although see Mahon and Caramazza, 2008).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…We think this opposition is visible in psycholinguistics, too. Conceptual and direct replication research is unfortunately very sparse (but see Nieuwland et al, 2017;Rommers, Meyer, & Huettig, 2013;Zwaan & Pecher, 2012; see also Jäger, Engelmann, & Vasishth, 2017), and even novel but incremental contributions are often considered insufficient for publication.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%