1989
DOI: 10.2190/g89v-30dn-eghf-dwn3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Occurrence Schemas, Context Ambiguity, and Hypnotic Responding

Abstract: We assessed the extent to which observers classified the behavior of a videotaped model as a goal-directed action or as an involuntary occurrence while varying the social context in which the model's behavior was embedded. Observers watched a model: a) respond to an arm levitation suggestion in a situation explicitly defined as hypnotic, b) respond to the same suggestion in a situation not defined as hypnotic, or c) attempt to swat a bothersome fly. In both open-ended testimony and on questionnaires, observers… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

1989
1989
2005
2005

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

2
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Alternatively, some subjects may have responded in goal-directed fashion to suggestions but remained unaware of having done so (Gorassini, 1988; Spanos, Salas, Bertrand, & Johnston, 1988). According to this notion, many subjects hold causal theories or schemas of hypnotic responding as involuntary occurrence.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Alternatively, some subjects may have responded in goal-directed fashion to suggestions but remained unaware of having done so (Gorassini, 1988; Spanos, Salas, Bertrand, & Johnston, 1988). According to this notion, many subjects hold causal theories or schemas of hypnotic responding as involuntary occurrence.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to this notion, many subjects hold causal theories or schemas of hypnotic responding as involuntary occurrence. Such schemas are likely to be strongly primed by cues in the hypnotic context (e.g., wording of suggestions; Spanos, et al, 1988). Once primed, these schemas may lead subjects to infer incorrectly that their goal-directed responses to suggestions were (or will be) automatic happenings.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite subjects' feelings of involuntariness, they in fact retain control of their hypnotic behavior. Social psychological models conceptualize hypnotic responding as scripted role enactment in which subjects modify their responses strategically in terms of shifting role demands (Sarbin & Coe, 1972; Spanos, 1986a, 1986b; Spanos, Salas, Bertrand, & Johnston, in press). Hypnotizable subjects are seen as active cognizers who are invested in meeting the requirements of hypnotic role behavior and are sensitively attuned to the broad demands of the testing context.…”
Section: Theories Of Hypnotic Involuntarinessmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to Kruglanski (1975), an action is commonly assumed to be determined by the actor's will, whereas an occurrence is largely independent of the will and is caused by factors other than the self. As social psychological theorists (Coe, 1978; Sarbin & Coe, 1979; Spanos et al, in press) have pointed out, people do not accept direct responsibility for occurrences or happenings. The tactic of disclaiming of responsibility is expressed in statements about everyday actions: “It happened to me,” “I was overcome,” and “I was carried away.” Hypnotic conduct, then, is not far removed from other spheres of life, where linguistic artifice and the disclaiming of agency buttress perceptions of involuntariness by transforming actions into the category of occurrences.…”
Section: A Social Cognitive Analysis Of Hypnotic Involuntarinessmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation