2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104917
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Oculomotor feedback rapidly reduces overt attentional capture

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Ref. 77 , 78 ). Since our aim was to isolate possible negative effects of the alert signal’s salience even under favorable conditions, we did not manipulate load, but kept the primary task simple and constant.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ref. 77 , 78 ). Since our aim was to isolate possible negative effects of the alert signal’s salience even under favorable conditions, we did not manipulate load, but kept the primary task simple and constant.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, workers could practice orienting to specific potential hazards when they come within a certain proximity, to the point that such orienting becomes a habitual attentional response. Such training could be coupled with adaptive attentional feedback (see Anderson & Mrkonja, 2021, 2022) to ensure the engagement of attentive behaviors that could become habitual with practice. Such training could even be paired with a system of reward contingencies for vigilance toward potential hazards; this might accentuate training benefits (see Anderson et al, 2011).…”
Section: Curbing Attention To Hazards Through Experiential Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Do the demands of finding and extracting relevant perceptual information, such as those involved in visual search, meaningfully contribute to the cognitive effort required by a task? Unlike memory-demanding cognitive tasks, people often conduct a visual search with limited awareness of what they have looked at (e.g., Adams & Gaspelin, 2020, 2021; Anderson & Mrkonja, 2021; Chen & Wyble, 2015; Horowitz & Wolfe, 1998; Theeuwes et al, 1998; Võ et al, 2016), maintaining a representation of the searched-for stimulus in active memory (Woodman et al, 2013) and engaging low-demand and potentially largely automatic attentional processes (see Anderson, 2018). Indeed, a number of involuntary, low-demand attentional processes support the selection of pertinent stimuli, which has been argued to reflect an adaptive attentional system that functions to minimize the need for controlled and effortful processes in the attainment of a desired state (Anderson, 2021).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%