1984
DOI: 10.1080/00222895.1984.10735327
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On Separability of and Interference Between Tracking Dimensions in Dual-Axis Tracking

Abstract: Time-shared tasks may conceivably be separable or integral. A case in which the question of separability seems quite relevant is dual-axis tracking. To test the interaction between tracking dimensions, we first studied whether they interfere with each other. Practiced subjects performed tracking on one or two axes, with or without feedback indicators and with or without a requirement to allocate resources unevenly between axes. They also performed with or without a concurrent binary classification of visually … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
8
0

Year Published

1985
1985
2010
2010

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
1
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This pattern of data captures the essence of the previous results, although it is not clear how one should interpret points that fall along the dotted lines, where the instructions to pay unequal attention to the two channels indicate no loss on the 80% channel but a lowering of performance on the 20% channel. This problem has been seen before (Gopher & Navon, 1980;Navon, Gopher, Chillag, & Spitz, 1984;Regan, 1982;Schneider & Fisk, 1982). Perhaps points along the arms of the square AOC represent a strategy in which the subject interprets the unbalanced instruction to mean that performance "should be" less on the low-attention channel and responds to it inefficiently, using less than the available capacity (Navon, 1985).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This pattern of data captures the essence of the previous results, although it is not clear how one should interpret points that fall along the dotted lines, where the instructions to pay unequal attention to the two channels indicate no loss on the 80% channel but a lowering of performance on the 20% channel. This problem has been seen before (Gopher & Navon, 1980;Navon, Gopher, Chillag, & Spitz, 1984;Regan, 1982;Schneider & Fisk, 1982). Perhaps points along the arms of the square AOC represent a strategy in which the subject interprets the unbalanced instruction to mean that performance "should be" less on the low-attention channel and responds to it inefficiently, using less than the available capacity (Navon, 1985).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A related theory of attentional interference is multiple resource theory, according to which different tasks draw on different resources (e.g., verbal vs. spatial WM), and thus, only tasks that require the same resources should interfere with each otherʼs processing (Navon, Gopher, Chillag, & Spitz, 1984;Gopher, Brickner, & Navon, 1982;Isreal, Chesney, Wickens, & Donchin, 1980;Wickens & Kessel, 1980). In a series of dual-task experiments, Kim, Kim, and Chun (2005)) have, for example, shown that limited-capacity resources can be tied up in a WM task and are subsequently unavailable to the processing of either targets or distractors in a Stroop task (Stroop, 1935).…”
Section: Effects Of Different Types Of Loadmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The hypothesis that concurrence is an option would be challenged, if it was shown that there was no evidence for concurrence even when concurrent processing was called for, namely when the performer had some reason for resorting to concurrent processing that outweighed its disadvantages. Concurrent processing might be preferable either when the tasks require sustained attention (e.g., retention in STM) or periodic monitoring (e.g., tracking) or when heavy time pressure makes queuing fatal to the accuracy of the postponed task (e.g., Gopher, Brickner, & Navon, 1982;Navon, Gopher, Chillag, & Spitz, 1984;Roldan, 1979;Sperling & Melchner, 1978;Tsang, Shaner, & Vidulich, 1995;reviews in Gopher, 1994;Gopher & Donchin, 1986;Wickens, 1984). However, advocates of the single-bottleneck notion have been expending most of their effort in demonstrating queuing in the overlapping tasks paradigm that does not call for concurrent processing.…”
Section: How Relevant Is the Paradigm Anyway?mentioning
confidence: 99%