1966
DOI: 10.1007/bf03393675
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the False Alarm Metaphor in Psychophysics

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

1967
1967
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Expectancies arising from direct imaginative suggestion can drive unusual experiences (e.g., hallucinations and apparently involuntary action) in a substantial proportion of the population (within and outside the context of 'hypnosis'; Braffman & Kirsch, 1999). Imaginative suggestion can be implicit rather than direct (Orne, 1959) and expectancies within scientific experiments can drive experiential change (e.g., gustatory hallucinations, Juhasz & Sarbin, 1966;psychedelic experiences, Heaton 1975; see Kirsch & Council, 1989). RHI response is predicted both by participant expectancies and by a stable trait measure of response to direct imaginative suggestion in a hypnotic context (hypnotisability).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Expectancies arising from direct imaginative suggestion can drive unusual experiences (e.g., hallucinations and apparently involuntary action) in a substantial proportion of the population (within and outside the context of 'hypnosis'; Braffman & Kirsch, 1999). Imaginative suggestion can be implicit rather than direct (Orne, 1959) and expectancies within scientific experiments can drive experiential change (e.g., gustatory hallucinations, Juhasz & Sarbin, 1966;psychedelic experiences, Heaton 1975; see Kirsch & Council, 1989). RHI response is predicted both by participant expectancies and by a stable trait measure of response to direct imaginative suggestion in a hypnotic context (hypnotisability).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is existing evidence that demand characteristics can drive experience in scientific experiments. For example, psychedelic experiences following placebo hallucinogen administration (Heaton, 1975;Olson et al, 2020) and hallucinated taste of salt in a psychophysical task Juhasz & Sarbin, 1966). It has previously been proposed that such effects may be driven by the same mechanisms as response to imaginative suggestion (see Kirsch & Council, 1989;Michael, Garry, and Kirsch (2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Clearly, Brain would not recommend commitment for these latter 43% who were utterly convinced of the genuineness of their gustatory "hallucinations." In fact, as we pointed out in our paper (29) such experimental results would not normally fall under the rubric of "hallucinations." In the ordinary psychological literature of today they would probably be called "false alarms."…”
Section: Vfimentioning
confidence: 82%
“…I n a recent experiment the authors (29) have demonstrated that a psychophysical-laboratory setting generates role demands of such strength as to produce a "salty" response to distilled water in 82% of a sample of college students. Furthermore, 43% of the sample indicated that they would testify at a murder trial that they had tasted salt.…”
Section: Vfimentioning
confidence: 99%