1993
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000900008503
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On the origins of denial negation

Abstract: These are the principal questions of this study: are the early fonctions of negation, such as REJECTION, and the later emerging DENIAL no developmentally related? And when do children start to deny? So far, evidence for the questions at hand has been almost exclusively observational. We decided on a simple elicitation procedure instead, asking 48 children (ages between 1;1 and 2;7) easy yes/no-questions. The most likely age range for the appearance of denial no was 1;8–2;1. Its error-free acquisition supports … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
39
0
3

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
1
39
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Lyon, Carrick, et al (2008) showed that children as young as 2.5 years who failed to accurately label true and false (T/F) statements as “good” or “bad,” and “truth” or “lie,” nevertheless reliably rejected false statements and accepted true statements. This is consistent with research concerning children’s ability to reject counterfactual statements, a skill demonstrated in children as young as 20 months (Hummer, Wimmer, & Antes, 1993; Pea, 1982). …”
Section: Children’s Understanding Of Truth and Lies: Research And Legsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Lyon, Carrick, et al (2008) showed that children as young as 2.5 years who failed to accurately label true and false (T/F) statements as “good” or “bad,” and “truth” or “lie,” nevertheless reliably rejected false statements and accepted true statements. This is consistent with research concerning children’s ability to reject counterfactual statements, a skill demonstrated in children as young as 20 months (Hummer, Wimmer, & Antes, 1993; Pea, 1982). …”
Section: Children’s Understanding Of Truth and Lies: Research And Legsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Although these two lines of research have yielded considerable insight into children's emerging understanding not only of the truth and lies but also of a broad range of related concepts, including inaccuracy, mistakes, jokes, and pretense (Hummer, Wimmer, & Antes, 1993; Koenig, Clement, & Harris, 2004; Siegal & Peterson, 1996, 1998; Sullivan, Winner, & Hopfield, 1995; Taylor, Lussier, & Maring, 2003), a basic question about children's conception of truthfulness and lying has gone unanswered—specifically, what role (if any) does the valence of the events about which children are questioned play in their early understanding?…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Denial showed the opposite pattern—it was rarely produced during the early sessions (4%) and increased in frequency in the later sessions (23%). David thus exhibited the same developmental pattern with respect to negation as hearing children learning English from their hearing parents (Bloom, 1970; Hummer et al, 1993). …”
Section: Negativesmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Bloom (1970) found that hearing children learning English initially express nonexistence and rejection in their negative sentences and only later express denial (see also Hummer, Wimmer & Antes, 1993). To determine whether David displayed a similar developmental pattern, we examined how often David expressed the three types of negative meanings during his first four sessions (ages 2;10 to 3;03) and during the later four sessions (ages 3;05 to 3;10).…”
Section: Negativesmentioning
confidence: 99%