2003
DOI: 10.1016/s0376-6357(02)00187-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Ontogeny has a phylogeny: background to adjunctive behaviors in pigeons and budgerigars

Abstract: Animals coping with operant conditioning tasks often show behaviors that are not recorded by keys, levers and similar response transducers. Nevertheless, these adjunctive behaviors should not be disposed of by classifying them as incidental. Often they are found to be at least partially influenced by the experimentally programmed contingencies, and under certain conditions they can in turn influence conditioned behaviors. Here we describe the occurrence and characteristics of two such behaviors, stimulus grasp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
5
0

Year Published

2005
2005
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
3
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is in line with the view that schedule-induced behaviors are operants (Killeen and Pellón, 2013), but goes against the categorization of impulsivity in two types (cognitive and motor) because it does not seem to be a difference between them in the present study. Schedule-induced drinking could be part of the same behavioral pattern that determines the choice of subjects, as reported by other authors (Cleaveland et al, 2003; Machado and Keen, 2003; López-Tolsa and Pellón, in preparation). Similar findings have been observed in DRL schedules (Segal and Holloway, 1963) and the peak procedure (Mattel and Portugal, 2007), tasks that both involve self-control.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…This is in line with the view that schedule-induced behaviors are operants (Killeen and Pellón, 2013), but goes against the categorization of impulsivity in two types (cognitive and motor) because it does not seem to be a difference between them in the present study. Schedule-induced drinking could be part of the same behavioral pattern that determines the choice of subjects, as reported by other authors (Cleaveland et al, 2003; Machado and Keen, 2003; López-Tolsa and Pellón, in preparation). Similar findings have been observed in DRL schedules (Segal and Holloway, 1963) and the peak procedure (Mattel and Portugal, 2007), tasks that both involve self-control.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Timberlake noted the Lamarckian coevolution of experimental apparatus and research programs and subsequently constructed behavior systems theory as a framework for integrating organisms' niche-appropriate behaviors into the conceptual milieu of the experimentalist (Timberlake, 1994;Timberlake & Lucas, 1985, 1989. A similar approach was mooted by Cleaveland, Jäger, Rößner and Delius (2003). It is the motor programs discussed by these researchers, these action patterns, that we argue are entrained by reinforcement (Davis & Hubbard, 1972;Palya & Zacny, 1980).…”
Section: Provenancementioning
confidence: 90%
“…This type of results leads to the question of what we are really measuring when we talk about temporal learning and/or time estimation (see Sanabria et al, 2009, for a case in which motivational variables, rather than timing, may account for better results in temporal tasks). As Cleaveland et al (2003) pointed out, scientists in the field of the analysis of behavior are usually only concerned with measuring one specific behavior like a lever press or a key peck, without paying attention to the actual organism behaving. Lejeune et al (1998) stated that schedule-induced behaviors do not mediate timed responses and proposed a two-process account for timed behavior.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Behaviors interact and compete during interreinforcement intervals (Baum, 2012; Killeen, 1975; Pellón & Killeen, 2015; Segal et al, 1965), and their distributions depend on the reinforcement schedule (Roper, 1978; Rosellini & Burdette, 1980), the animal species used (Millenson et al, 1977), and the complexity and size of the environment in which the organisms are tested (Skuban & Richardson, 1975; Staddon & Ayres, 1975). Nevertheless, once a specific pattern of behaviors is developed, it tends to occur in a semi-invariant way (i.e., in the same order, for similar periods of time) during most of the interreinforcement intervals if all the environmental conditions remain constant (Baum & Grace, 2020; Cleaveland et al, 2003; Lawler & Cohen, 1992; Ruiz et al, 2016; Segal & Holloway, 1963; Staddon & Ayres, 1975; Staddon & Simmelhag, 1971).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%