The Verb Phrase in English 2013
DOI: 10.1017/cbo9781139060998.012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Operator and negative contraction in spoken British English: a change in progress

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This explanation has had a noteworthy impact on both subsequent research into the evolution of negative/polarity indefinites in the history of English and on the development of theories of morphosyntactic change. The lexical effect reported by Tottie (1991a) has been replicated in diverse datasets of English (Childs et al, 2015, forthcoming; Varela Pérez, 2014). For example, in the comparative study of English spoken in Canada, including Toronto (using the TEA), Belleville (Tagliamonte, 2003–2006), the United Kingdom (using the York English Corpus [Tagliamonte, 1998] and North East England [Corrigan, Buchstaller, Mearns, & Hermann, 2010–2012; Tagliamonte, 1998, 2003–2006]), Childs et al (2015, forthcoming) reproduced the same lexical effects and roughly the same construction hierarchy as Tottie, as in Table 2 1…”
Section: Lexical Diffusion and The Emergence Of Any Polarity Itemsmentioning
confidence: 81%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This explanation has had a noteworthy impact on both subsequent research into the evolution of negative/polarity indefinites in the history of English and on the development of theories of morphosyntactic change. The lexical effect reported by Tottie (1991a) has been replicated in diverse datasets of English (Childs et al, 2015, forthcoming; Varela Pérez, 2014). For example, in the comparative study of English spoken in Canada, including Toronto (using the TEA), Belleville (Tagliamonte, 2003–2006), the United Kingdom (using the York English Corpus [Tagliamonte, 1998] and North East England [Corrigan, Buchstaller, Mearns, & Hermann, 2010–2012; Tagliamonte, 1998, 2003–2006]), Childs et al (2015, forthcoming) reproduced the same lexical effects and roughly the same construction hierarchy as Tottie, as in Table 2 1…”
Section: Lexical Diffusion and The Emergence Of Any Polarity Itemsmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Tottie's analysis of the patterns shown in Table 1 involves two distinct propositions. The first proposal, which is shared implicitly or explicitly by most works on the emergence of any indefinites in the history of English, is that the newer polarity item form ( any ) is in the process of replacing the older negative quantifier form ( no ) in all postverbal syntactic positions (Childs et al, 2015, forthcoming; Mitchell, 1985; Nevalainen, 1998, 2009; Smith, 2001; Varela Perez, 2014; among many others). Under this assumption, the results in Table 1 appear to show that the change is diffusing across individual lexical items/constructions, being closer to completion with lexical verbs than with existential constructions.…”
Section: Lexical Diffusion and The Emergence Of Any Polarity Itemsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This means that the use of NQs in post-verbal position is a syntactically marked option. In addition, previous corpus-based research has shown that negation with NQs is favored with BE/HAVE, while negation with a negative marker and any-PIs is favored with lexical verbs (Tottie 1991a, b;Varela Pérez 2014;Childs et al 2015;Wallage 2017).…”
Section: Conflict Of Interest Statementmentioning
confidence: 93%