“…This approach (the main theoretical and methodological framework of which are summarized in Bianchi and Savardi, 2008a) has led to various experimental explorations of the abilities of adults and children to recognize whether certain configurations are "opposite" as compared to "different" or "similar" and to also produce opposite configurations. These explorations referred to various perceptual domains and contents and involved, for example, simple visual configurations (e.g., Bianchi and Savardi, 2006;Schepis et al, 2009;Savardi et al, 2010;Bianchi et al, 2017a), everyday objects and/or environments (e.g., Bianchi et al, 2011Bianchi et al, , 2013Bianchi et al, , 2017c, human body postures (e.g., Bianchi and Savardi, 2008b;Bianchi et al, 2014), and acoustic stimuli (e.g., Biassoni, 2009;Bianchi et al, 2017b). The results emerging from these studies indicated that (a) the participants were consistent in defining the point along a dimension at which a property stops being perceived as pertaining to one extreme of the dimension (e.g., near on the dimension near/far) and starts to be perceived as an instance of the opposite property (e.g., in this case far) or of an intermediate region with variations that are not perceived as pertaining to either one pole or the other (e.g., neither near nor far); (b) two configurations are perceived to be opposites when they show a maximum contrast in terms of a perceptually salient property-usually regarding the orientation or direction of the configuration-within a condition of otherwise overall invariance (some examples are provided in Figure 2); and (c) due evidently to this close connection between the perception of opposition and the perception of opposite orientation in an overall invariant configuration, opposition is also perceived in configurations involving mirror symmetry, including people's perception of their own body reflection in a plane mirror.…”