1966
DOI: 10.1037/h0023007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Optimality of perceptual decision criteria.

Abstract: J-This paper is based upon a dissertation submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Colorado in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD degree. The research was in part performed while the writer held a predoctoral fellowship from the National Institute of Mental Health. The writer wishes to thank K. E. Davis and D. Mason for their advice and assistance, and is especially indebted to F. J. Todd, his dissertation chairman.8 Now at the University of Denver.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

5
69
1

Year Published

1968
1968
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 62 publications
(75 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
5
69
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, a series ofprevious studies (Healy & Kubovy, 1981;Lee & Janke, 1965;Ulehla, 1966) suggests that conservative cutoffplacement is likely to result when category base-rates are unequal. Why is extreme cutoff placement so prevalent in one set of studies and conservative cutoffplacement so prevalent in another set of studies?…”
Section: Conservative Versus Extreme Cutoff Placementmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, a series ofprevious studies (Healy & Kubovy, 1981;Lee & Janke, 1965;Ulehla, 1966) suggests that conservative cutoffplacement is likely to result when category base-rates are unequal. Why is extreme cutoff placement so prevalent in one set of studies and conservative cutoffplacement so prevalent in another set of studies?…”
Section: Conservative Versus Extreme Cutoff Placementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Maddox outlined several differences between the two sets of studies that might account for the discrepant findings. In many cases, the previous studies (Healy & Kubovy, 1981;Lee & Janke, 1965;Ulehla, 1966) used fairly inexperienced observers, analyzed the data collapsed across observers, and used unidimensional stimuli. Maddox (1995;Maddox & Bohil, 1998) and the present study used highly experienced observers, analyzed the data at the individual observer level, and used twodimensional stimuli.…”
Section: Conservative Versus Extreme Cutoff Placementmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, if the benefit of a correct Category A response is 3 points, the benefit of a correct Category B response is 1 point, and the cost of an incorrect response is 0 points (referred to as a 3:1 zerocost condition, because no loss of points is associated with an incorrect response), the optimal classifier decision criterion, b o = 3, maximizes long-run reward. Under these conditions, observed decision criterion values fall somewhere between this reward-maximizing criterion (b o = 3) and the accuracy-maximizing criterion (b 5 1; see, e.g., Green & Swets, 1966;Healy & Kubovy, 1981;Kubovy & Healy, 1977;Lee & Janke, 1964, 1965Lee & Zentall, 1966;Ulehla, 1966). This is termed conservative cutoff placement, because the decision criterion is not shifted far enough toward the optimal value.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The research described here included two tests of the integration model in a type of visual discrimination task previously used to investigate other aspects of SOT (Ulehla, 1966). This task involved the judgment of the direction of tilt (right vs left) of lines rotated Y2 deg from the vertical as defined by an enclosing square.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%