2008
DOI: 10.1007/s10764-008-9262-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Organization of Group Members at Departure Is Driven by Social Structure in Macaca

Abstract: Researchers have often explained order of progression of group members during joint movement in terms of the influence of ecological pressures but rarely that of social constraints. We studied the order of joining by group members to a movement in semifree-ranging macaques with contrasting social systems: 1 group of Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) and 1 group of rhesus macaques (M. mulatta). We used network metrics to understand roles and associations among individuals. The way the macaques joined a movemen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
173
3
3

Year Published

2010
2010
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 138 publications
(188 citation statements)
references
References 72 publications
9
173
3
3
Order By: Relevance
“…In these conditions, the two new sub groups will be more composed on kin related individuals (Chepko Sade and Sade, 1979;Van Horn et al, 2007). This influence did not affect, however, our results, since groups having an influence of kinship can also be represented using random networks as the ones we used in this study (Flack et al, 2006;McCowan et al, 2008;Sueur and Petit, 2008;Kasper and Voelkl, 2009;Ramos Fernandez et al, 2009). Grooming a specific and low number of partners could also be of interest when managing time (individuals do not need to change their grooming time when group size increases) but could also have implications when considering cognitive capacities.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…In these conditions, the two new sub groups will be more composed on kin related individuals (Chepko Sade and Sade, 1979;Van Horn et al, 2007). This influence did not affect, however, our results, since groups having an influence of kinship can also be represented using random networks as the ones we used in this study (Flack et al, 2006;McCowan et al, 2008;Sueur and Petit, 2008;Kasper and Voelkl, 2009;Ramos Fernandez et al, 2009). Grooming a specific and low number of partners could also be of interest when managing time (individuals do not need to change their grooming time when group size increases) but could also have implications when considering cognitive capacities.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…We did not set networks as scale free since recent primate studies showed that social networks were not scale free (i.e. with a power distribution of links) but random (see Flack et al, 2006;McCowan et al, 2008;Sueur and Petit, 2008;Kasper and Voelkl, 2009;Ramos Fernandez et al, 2009 for studies on primate social networks; see Wasserman and Faust, 1994 for social network theory). We can observe on Fig.…”
Section: Theoretical Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…At the dyadic level, individuals can reliably predict the behaviour of the other. At the group level, affiliative relationships underpin various patterns of social life such as coalitions, collective movements or information transmission (Chepko- Sade & Sade 1979;Silk et al 2004;Sueur & Petit 2008;Voelkl & Noë 2008). Coalitions allow individuals to win in social competition or to gain support in collective decisions by recruiting mates and kin (Chapais 1995); moreover, some key individuals can favour the spread of information or disease by their central position in social networks (Sueur et al 2011).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%