2006
DOI: 10.1523/jneurosci.1100-06.2006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Orientation-Specific Computation in Stereoscopic Vision

Abstract: The left and right eyes receive subtly different images from a visual scene. Binocular disparities of retinal image locations are correlated with variation in the depth of objects in the scene and make stereoscopic depth perception possible. Disparity stereoscopically specifies a stimulus; changing the stimulus in a way that conserves its disparity leaves the stimulus stereoscopically unchanged. Therefore, a person's ability to use stereo to see the depth separating any two objects should depend only on the di… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
22
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
1
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Also, the plaids’ contrast envelopes, despite their fixed disparities, were not useful as reference stimuli against which to judge the depth of the test plaid. The disparities appearing in this study are far smaller than the threshold disparity for an isolated grating patch, with an envelope disparity of zero, having the same carrier spatial frequency as used here (Farell, 2006). The observed values in that case are characteristic of absolute, not relative, disparity thresholds.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 69%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Also, the plaids’ contrast envelopes, despite their fixed disparities, were not useful as reference stimuli against which to judge the depth of the test plaid. The disparities appearing in this study are far smaller than the threshold disparity for an isolated grating patch, with an envelope disparity of zero, having the same carrier spatial frequency as used here (Farell, 2006). The observed values in that case are characteristic of absolute, not relative, disparity thresholds.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…This is an advantage of coding disparity along a stimulus-independent direction. The visual system does not do this with 1-D stimuli like gratings, but rather codes disparity and orientation jointly (Farell, 2006; Farell et al, 2009). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Horizontal disparity supplies the common metric for perceived depth in most laboratory studies and theories of stereopsis. However, two 1-D stimuli, if they have different orientations and identical horizontal disparities, will have different effective disparities: The two stimuli can appear at different depths relative to the same reference stimulus Farell, 2006;Farell, Chai, & Fernandez, 2009;Ito, 2005). By dissociating perceived depth from horizontal disparity, 1-D stimuli allow us to create a set of stimulus pairs whose depth relations, taken together, violate transitivity, despite the normal appearance of each pair when viewed individually.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The result is that stereoacuity and perceived depth for 1-D patterns vary with stimulus orientation, a fact known for many years but open to diverse interpretations (Blake, Camisa, & Antoinetti, 1976;Ebenholtz & Walchli, 1965;Farell & Ahuja, 1996;Friedman, Kaye, & Richards, 1978;Morgan & Castet, 1997;Ogle, 1955; see Howard & Rogers, 2002). In general, the psychophysical effects of 1-D stimulus orientation are consistent with an effective disparity that has a direction perpendicular to the orientation Farell, 1998Farell, , 2006Morgan & Castet, 1997;Patel, Bedell, & Sampat, 2006;Patel et al, 2003;Quaia et al, 2013), though the physiological evidence is mixed (e.g., Cumming, 2002;Durand, Celebrini, & Trotter, 2007;Maske, Yamane, & Bishop, 1986).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 88%