2021
DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abe8212
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Output planning at the input stage in visual working memory

Abstract: Working memory serves as the buffer between past sensations and future behavior, making it vital to understand not only how we encode and retain sensory information in memory but also how we plan for its upcoming use. We ask when prospective action goals emerge alongside the encoding and retention of visual information in working memory. We show that prospective action plans do not emerge gradually during memory delays but are brought into memory early, in tandem with sensory encoding. This action encoding (i)… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

11
131
3

Year Published

2021
2021
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

4
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 72 publications
(145 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
(112 reference statements)
11
131
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Additionally, we found that information about prospective actions declined toward the end of the delay period, dropping below chance just before the response disk appeared (Figure 3B). This time course is consistent with a transient signal related to the initial formation of a prospective memory representation, and aligns with recent EEG findings in human motor cortex following a retro-cue in a visual WM task (Boettcher et al, 2021).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Additionally, we found that information about prospective actions declined toward the end of the delay period, dropping below chance just before the response disk appeared (Figure 3B). This time course is consistent with a transient signal related to the initial formation of a prospective memory representation, and aligns with recent EEG findings in human motor cortex following a retro-cue in a visual WM task (Boettcher et al, 2021).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…As a consequence, the hand required for responding was directly linked to the angle of the bar that was probed. This builds on previous tasks from our lab (Boettcher et al, 2021;van Ede et al, 2019), though we note that the specifics of this response implementation were not critical to the current study. Once participants started rotating the dial, they were given only limited time (4000 ms) to complete the angle reproduction.…”
Section: Task and Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 63%
“…Besides calling on attentional control processes and mental workspaces that may have been concurrently active for working memory (e.g., Bae & Luck, 2019;Barrouillet et al, 2007;Souza & Oberauer, 2017), interrupters -which required a manual response -may also have directly affected the preparation of memory-guided actions. In our task, items were associated with specific actions which would allow action plans to be coactivated together with visual representations (Boettcher et al, 2021;van Ede et al, 2019). As such, interrupters may have additionally interfered with action plans in working memory, yielding a more detrimental effect on performance than was elicited by visual distractors not requiring any manual response.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The interaction with task relevance suggests that stimulus processing is only biased when it is primed for later use in upcoming behaviour. Such an interplay of sensory and action systems has been shown to affect the encoding of visual features (Boettcher, Gresch, Nobre, & van Ede, 2021; Myers, Stokes, & Nobre, 2017; van Ede, Chekroud, Stokes, & Nobre, 2019). Circuits involved in the previous response could interact with motor encoding of the subsequent trial.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%