2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.02.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
155
0
4

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
3
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 182 publications
(166 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
7
155
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…B 371: 20150071 consequences of social learning [17,18,[43][44][45][46][47]. Approaching the same question from a developmental perspective, as already noted above, children do not overimitate until their second year, and growing evidence suggests they indeed do so because of social and/or normative reasons [29,[43][44][45][46][47][48][49][50][51][52][53][54]. Finally, the fact that overimitation occurs across multiple contrasting cultural contexts [33][34][35] indicates that the sociocultural environment serving as a resource for acquiring human-specific overimitation in social learning is shared across most human communities.…”
Section: Social Learningmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…B 371: 20150071 consequences of social learning [17,18,[43][44][45][46][47]. Approaching the same question from a developmental perspective, as already noted above, children do not overimitate until their second year, and growing evidence suggests they indeed do so because of social and/or normative reasons [29,[43][44][45][46][47][48][49][50][51][52][53][54]. Finally, the fact that overimitation occurs across multiple contrasting cultural contexts [33][34][35] indicates that the sociocultural environment serving as a resource for acquiring human-specific overimitation in social learning is shared across most human communities.…”
Section: Social Learningmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…Although some accounts of overimitation have explained this puzzling behavior in terms of brute cognitive limitations (e.g., Lyons, Young, & Keil, 2007), recent work suggests there may be more to the story. Even when it is made salient to them that certain actions are not instrumental to achieving a goal, children still overimitate and protest third parties who fail to overimitate (Kenward, 2012;Keupp, Behne and Rakoczy, 2013). Hence, in these situations, it seems that children may be drawing conclusions about what is prescriptively appropriate on the basis of what they infer is conventional or normal.…”
Section: Learning Morality From Normalitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A voluminous literature in psychology and sociology demonstrates that humans are innately attuned to the informational and normative cues in others' behaviors, even when interaction is transient or superficial (Cialdini and Goldstein 2004;Miller and Prentice 2016). Experiments by evolutionary psychologists, for example, show that children imitate unfamiliar adults' behaviors both as a means to reduce uncertainty (Lyons, Young, and Keil 2007) and out of concern for normative compliance (Kenward 2012). This sensitivity to others' behaviors generalizes to a variety of contexts.…”
Section: The Cultural Conductivity Of Superficial Interactionsmentioning
confidence: 99%