2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2019.102207
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Overconfidence over the lifespan: Evidence from Germany

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
16
0
2

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 81 publications
0
16
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…sures, absolute and relative health perception biases, whose empirical measurement we borrow from the growing literature on overconfidence (c.f. Tiefenbeck et al, 2018;Friehe and Pannenberg, 2019). Absolute perception biases are biased perceptions of own health, whereas relative perception biases are biased perceptions relative to population health.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…sures, absolute and relative health perception biases, whose empirical measurement we borrow from the growing literature on overconfidence (c.f. Tiefenbeck et al, 2018;Friehe and Pannenberg, 2019). Absolute perception biases are biased perceptions of own health, whereas relative perception biases are biased perceptions relative to population health.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This survey question has been successfully tested in other contexts. For example, using the same format, respondents in the Swiss "Amphiro" study were asked about their income position, their water use, and their knowledge of energy conservation(Tiefenbeck et al, 2018;Friehe and Pannenberg, 2019).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Gervais and Odean (2001) found that traders may learn to be overconfident not because they become better at trading but because they give credit of their success to overconfidence: "Overconfidence does not make traders wealthy, but the process of becoming wealthy can make traders overconfident". Merkle (2017) has corroborated the learning-to-be-overconfident hypothesis; Friehe and Pannenberg (2019) show that overconfidence increases with experience. Gentile et al (2016), for the Italian market, confirm that "Older and wealthier individuals are more likely to be overconfident".…”
Section: Crt and Cognitive Biasesmentioning
confidence: 60%
“…Put differently, in a class of 100 students, the student with the 50 th rank always has the median performance, regardless of whether the class as a whole is weak or strong. Our measure of overplacement is commonly used in research on confidence (e.g., C. Anderson et al, 2012;Belmi, Neale, Reiff, & Ulfe, 2019;Ehrlinger, Mitchum, & Dweck, 2016;Emich, 2014;Friehe & Pannenberg, 2019;Muthukrishna et al, 2018). To calculate discrepancy between self-estimated placement and actual placement, we simply subtracted actual from estimated placement; but for tests of covariation involving overplacement, we used the residuals when regressing selfestimated placement on actual placement, which capture aspects of beliefs that cannot be explained by true performance, consistent with existing approaches (C. Anderson et al, 2012;Cronbach & Furby, 1970;Dubois, 1957;John & Robins, 1994a; see Supplemental Materials for expanded discussion on calculating discrepancy).…”
Section: Materials and Procedurementioning
confidence: 99%