2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.08.015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Package color saturation and food healthfulness perceptions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
85
0
6

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 103 publications
(93 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
2
85
0
6
Order By: Relevance
“…Because of these associations, consumers who have active health goals prefer products with light‐colored packaging, whereas those who have an active indulgence goal avoid light‐colored packaging. Mead and Richerson () showed that packaging color saturation can bias consumers’ food perceptions. Consumers rely on a judgment heuristic (Kahneman & Frederick, ), associating vivid, highly color‐saturated food packaging with unhealthful food.…”
Section: Conceptual Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Because of these associations, consumers who have active health goals prefer products with light‐colored packaging, whereas those who have an active indulgence goal avoid light‐colored packaging. Mead and Richerson () showed that packaging color saturation can bias consumers’ food perceptions. Consumers rely on a judgment heuristic (Kahneman & Frederick, ), associating vivid, highly color‐saturated food packaging with unhealthful food.…”
Section: Conceptual Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is, therefore, important to verify that the effect of au naturel packaging color on WTP is robust to different lightness levels. Moreover, Study 4 used stimuli with equal levels of saturation, which a recent contribution suggested as a relevant determinant of the perceived healthfulness of products (Mead & Richerson, ). Whereas Studies 2 and 3 used as non au naturel colors red and orange—which have saturation and lightness levels (the latter measured through the brightness dimension of the HSB model) that are intrinsically different from beige—Study 4 adopted blue as non au naturel color, keeping saturation levels equal across conditions and explicitly manipulating lightness.…”
Section: Hypothesis Testingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, package shape (i.e., angular versus rounded packages, anthropomorphizing a package shape to mirror an ideal human body shape) and package color (i.e., less saturated versus more saturated colors) drive taste beliefs and even actual taste experiences [7,19,20]. Package colors can also influence the product's perceived healthiness, as research established highly saturated colors (versus lowly saturated colors) to improve health perceptions [21]. Moreover, package size influences quality beliefs in that smaller (versus larger) packages are thought to be of higher quality [22].…”
Section: Food Package Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To date, most of color research in business domain primarily focused on color hue (Chylinski, Northey, & Ngo, ; Kareklas, Brunel, & Coulter, ; Lee, Fujita, Deng, & Unnava, ; Puzakova, Kwak, Ramanathan, & Rocereto, ; Seo & Scammon, ; Wauters, Brengman, & Mahama, ) and value (Hagtvedt & Brasel, ; Hsieh, Chiu, Tang, & Lee, ; Mai, Symmank, & Seeberg‐Elverfeldt, ; Reinoso‐Carvalho, Dakduk, Wagemans, & Spence, ). However, the explorations on the effects of color saturation (Hagtvedt & Brasel, ; Mead & Richerson, ), a seemingly most important color dimension out of the three in terms of triggering peoples’ emotional responses (Suk & Irtel, ), remains relatively scarce (see Labrecque et al, for review). The recent study by Hagtvedt and Brasel () found converging evidence that high (vs. low) color saturation would lead to larger (vs. smaller) perceived size.…”
Section: Hypotheses and Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%