Why is pain still under-treated in the emergency department? Two new hypotheses Bioethics, 2016; 30(3):195-202 © 2015 John Wiley & Sons Ltd This is the peer reviewed version of the following article: Drew Carter, Paul Sendziuk, Jaklin A. Eliott, Annette Braunack-Mayer Why is pain still under-treated in the emergency department? Two new hypotheses Bioethics, 2016; 30(3):195-202
Publishing in a subscription based journal
Accepted (peer-reviewed) VersionSelf-archiving of the accepted version is subject to an embargo period of 12-24 months. The embargo period is 12 months for scientific, technical, and medical (STM) journals and 24 months for social science and humanities (SSH) journals following publication of the final article.The accepted version may be placed on:• the author's personal website • the author's company/institutional repository or archive • certain not for profit subject-based repositories such as PubMed Central as listed below Articles may be deposited into repositories on acceptance, but access to the article is subject to the embargo period. We canvass the literature testifying to this problem, the reasons why this problem is so important, and then some of the main hypotheses that have been advanced in explanation of the problem. We then argue for the plausibility of two new hypotheses: pain's under-treatment in the ED partly owes to (1) an epistemic preference for signs over symptoms on the part of some practitioners, and (2) some ED practices that themselves worsen pain by increasing patients' anxiety and fear. Our argumentation includes the 2 following logic. Some ED practitioners depart from formal guidance in basing their acute pain assessments on observable features rather than on patient reports of pain. This is potentially due to an epistemic preference for signs over symptoms which aims to circumvent intentional and/or unintentional misrepresentation on the part of patients. However, conducting pain assessments in line with this epistemic preference contributes to the under-treatment of pain in at least three respects, which we detail. Moreover, it may do little to help the practitioner circumvent any intentional misrepresentation on the part of the patient, as we explain.Second, we examine at least four ED practices that may be contributing to the under-treatment of pain by increasing patient anxiety and fear, which can worsen pain. These practices include the failure to provide orienting information and the partial objectification of patients required to problemsolve along lines pre-established by modern medical science. We conclude by touching on some potential solutions for ED practice.