2015
DOI: 10.3366/word.2015.0081
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Paradigmatic complexity in pidgins and creoles

Abstract: The last decade has seen increasing attention paid to questions of grammatical complexity, in particular regarding the extent to which some languages can be said to be more ‘complex’ than others, whether globally or with respect to particular subsystems. Creoles have featured prominently in these debates, with various authors arguing that they are particularly simple when set against non-creoles, with an apparent lack of overt morphology in creoles often cited as one of the ways in which their grammars are esp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
13
2
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
1
13
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We will return to this in section 6.1.1. Overall, our findings do not support global level-specific regularisation biases as an explanation for the apparent asymmetry between simplification of morphological and word order variation hinted at in the literature on language learning and pidgin formation (Bichakjian 1988;Drechsel 1981;Good 2015). Instead, this study suggests that variation is reduced to a similar degree across linguistic levels, with any asymmetry likely due to differing complexity of linguistic paradigms or other features of the contact (or second) languages (Ansaldo et al 2007;Mufwene 2008).…”
Section: Tablecontrasting
confidence: 87%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…We will return to this in section 6.1.1. Overall, our findings do not support global level-specific regularisation biases as an explanation for the apparent asymmetry between simplification of morphological and word order variation hinted at in the literature on language learning and pidgin formation (Bichakjian 1988;Drechsel 1981;Good 2015). Instead, this study suggests that variation is reduced to a similar degree across linguistic levels, with any asymmetry likely due to differing complexity of linguistic paradigms or other features of the contact (or second) languages (Ansaldo et al 2007;Mufwene 2008).…”
Section: Tablecontrasting
confidence: 87%
“…These studies implicitly assume uniform mechanisms and processes behind regularisation; no study has directly compared how learners respond to variation at different levels. However, research on natural language learning and formation suggests that morphological and syntactic variation may not be treated in the same way by learners (Good 2015;Siegel 2006;Slobin 1986).…”
Section: Level-specific Effects On Regularisationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In this paper, I take issue with both sides of the debate. I claim that, on the one hand, grammars are not robustly transmitted during the emergence of creole languages, since certain core aspects of language (e.g., bound morphology and tones) tend to be reduced during creolization (Siegel, 2003(Siegel, , 2006Good, 2012Good, , 2015Parkvall, 2008;Jansson et al, 2015;Saldana et al, 2018); at the same time, I also argue that creoles are not the simplest languages in the world, since in other aspects of their grammars (e.g., syntax, phonology and semantics), they may inherit a fair number of overt distinctions, which make them quite complex, from an overall comparative perspective.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%