2018
DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0197
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Parasite insight: assessing fitness costs, infection risks and foraging benefits relating to gastrointestinal nematodes in wild mammalian herbivores

Abstract: Mammalian herbivores are typically infected by parasitic nematodes, which are acquired through direct, faecal-oral transmission. These parasites can cause significant production losses in domestic livestock, but much less is known about impacts on wild mammalian hosts. We review three elements of parasitism from the host's perspective: fitness costs of infection, risks of infection during foraging and benefits of nutritious pasture. The majority of wildlife studies have been observational, but experimental man… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
43
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 80 publications
1
43
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, bonobos did not avoid contaminated food that was put in contact with conspecific faeces in front of them, if the latter got covered and the food alone was presented to them [15]. Finally, it is also likely that a three-way trade-off between disgust (infection risk), hunger (energy or nutrient requirements) [11,18] and information acquisition may mediate the expression of avoidance, potentially in divergent ways depending on the type of food reward and the type of contaminant or test stimulus presented, and of course the sensory modality being invoked.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similarly, bonobos did not avoid contaminated food that was put in contact with conspecific faeces in front of them, if the latter got covered and the food alone was presented to them [15]. Finally, it is also likely that a three-way trade-off between disgust (infection risk), hunger (energy or nutrient requirements) [11,18] and information acquisition may mediate the expression of avoidance, potentially in divergent ways depending on the type of food reward and the type of contaminant or test stimulus presented, and of course the sensory modality being invoked.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Among substrates known to harbour infectious agents, faeces are known to elicit avoidance behaviour in a wide range of animal species, including insects [8], rodents [9], ungulates [10], marsupials [11] and proboscids [12]. This is for good reason, as a multitude of infectious organisms (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, previous studies on Rhodesian elephants have recorded much higher mean egg burden reaching 2072 with a range of 512-4382 [5]. Faecal egg counts or egg burden are often used to assess parasite burdens but have inherent pitfalls as they are subject to numerous variables that confound cause-effect relationships [43]. The few studies that have ever correlated egg burden to worm burdens have had variable outcomes [44][45][46][47].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These factors did not exactly mirror the transmission routes of infection but, instead, reflected different kinds of behavioural tasks involved in parasite avoidance. These six strategies for parasite avoidance may be universal, and examples in many other species can be found throughout this special edition, for example, fish avoid conspecifics of atypical appearance or with lesions [22], nematodes and primates avoid contaminated foods [18,20], mice avoid sex with infected conspecifics [19], bees are hygienic [21] and birds and ruminants avoid unhygienic environments [17,23]. Heterospecific avoidance behaviour has been rarely reported in the animal literature but could be expected to occur when other species serve as vectors or intermediate hosts of specific parasites.…”
Section: (B) Mechanismsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A further pattern emerging from this special issue is a general shift in the field from anecdotal reports of observed behaviour to the experimental testing of hypotheses, which has proven possible in captive species as diverse as bees [21] and bonobos [20], and, in some cases, in the wild as well [22,23]. It is also exciting to see how cross-disciplinary collaboration is bearing fruit, for example, where geneticists, cell biologists and behavioural scientists are collaborating to provide a complete picture of nematode infection avoidance behaviour [18].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%