Parental Care in Mammals 1981
DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4613-3150-6_7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Parent and Infant Attachment in Mammals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
77
0
4

Year Published

1989
1989
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 106 publications
(83 citation statements)
references
References 231 publications
(246 reference statements)
2
77
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…2; Lévy et al, 1991;. Even a few minutes of contact at birth are sufficient for does to maintain maternal responsiveness for 2-3 hr without their neonate (Gubernick, 1981;Klopfer et al, 1964). Thus, the sensitive period can be regarded as a phase during which maternal responsiveness is sustained by the experience that the mother gains while interacting with her neonate.…”
Section: The Sensitive Period Sensitive Period Versus Critical Periodmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…2; Lévy et al, 1991;. Even a few minutes of contact at birth are sufficient for does to maintain maternal responsiveness for 2-3 hr without their neonate (Gubernick, 1981;Klopfer et al, 1964). Thus, the sensitive period can be regarded as a phase during which maternal responsiveness is sustained by the experience that the mother gains while interacting with her neonate.…”
Section: The Sensitive Period Sensitive Period Versus Critical Periodmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Monozygotic twins, however, share much closer signatures, indicating some genotypic basis of olfactory identity (Romeyer et al, 1993b). Transmission of odors from the dam to the neonate, known as maternal labeling (Alexander, Stevens, & Bradley, 1989;Gubernick, 1981), is not critical for the initial establishment of maternal selectivity (Alexander et al, 1986;Poindron et al, 1988;Romeyer et al, 1993a). Nonetheless, it can contribute to the development of odor differences between neonates, for example, in young with initially very similar signatures (e.g., monozygotic twins; Romeyer et al, 1993b).…”
Section: Sensory Cues Involved In the Establishment Of Maternal Selecmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Soon after birth, the female will learn phenotypic features of the infant (Gubemick 1981) which usually results in reliable subsequent identification. However, even at this stage, evidence from Macaca nemestrina suggests that a female may employ a `rule of thumb' to identify her offspring, since if mother and infant are separated after birth, upon reunion 3-7 days later, mothers preferred the younger of two infants available, regardless of whether or not it was their own birth infant (Gubernick 1981). It is notable that the infant itself may play an active role in `who becomes the mother', as McGrew illustrates;…”
Section: Female-offspringmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are significant benefits to the infant from physical `contact' itself, such as warmth, and shelter both during waking hours and whilst asleep (together), and even provision of movement and physical stimulation (Gubernick 1981). During these periods of close contact, the female will express a range of more active social behaviours including; nursing with milk, carrying, cleaning and grooming, responding to signs of the infant's distress (e. g. in the form of vocalisations from the infant).…”
Section: Female-offspringmentioning
confidence: 99%