1994
DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1994.tb00261.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pattern of Development of the Callosal Transfer of Visual Information to Cortical Areas 17 and 18 in the Cat

Abstract: The aim of this study was to investigate the development of visual callosal transfer in the normally reared cat. Two- to nine-week-old kittens and adults (used as controls) underwent section of the optic chiasm. Three days later, the animals were placed under anesthesia and paralysed; unit activities were recorded from visual cortical areas 17 and 18 and from the white matter in one hemisphere. The units were tested for their responses to visual stimulation of each eye successively. Out of 1036 recorded neuron… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
35
0

Year Published

1995
1995
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
4
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This conclusion is comparable to conclusions reached in similar studies on areas 17 and 18 (Berlucchi and Rizzolatti, 1968;Lepore and Guillemot, 1982;Lepore etal., 1992;Milleret, 1994), 19 (Antonini et at., 1985;Guillemot et al, 1993) and the suprasylvian regions (Antonini et al, 1983), attesting to the apparent similarity of receptive field properties for gross spatial features (receptive field size, orientation and directional selectivities, position, etc.) derived from these two routes of activation.…”
Section: Functional Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…This conclusion is comparable to conclusions reached in similar studies on areas 17 and 18 (Berlucchi and Rizzolatti, 1968;Lepore and Guillemot, 1982;Lepore etal., 1992;Milleret, 1994), 19 (Antonini et at., 1985;Guillemot et al, 1993) and the suprasylvian regions (Antonini et al, 1983), attesting to the apparent similarity of receptive field properties for gross spatial features (receptive field size, orientation and directional selectivities, position, etc.) derived from these two routes of activation.…”
Section: Functional Implicationsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…the midline, but that this may not happen to the same degree in areas that are involved primarily with pattern recognition and high-acuity vision. It is unclear whether the biased topographies appear gradually, as a result of postnatal development (e.g., by a process of ''pruning'' of afferents similar to that described for callosal connections; Milleret et al, 1994), or are genetically ''preprogrammed'' by natural selection. Future studies aimed at defining the timetable of postnatal development of the flying fox visual system may be relevant to this question, given that these animals do not start to fly until the second or third postnatal month.…”
Section: Biased Representation Of the Visual Fieldmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Recordings from split-chiasm cats indicated, in addition to matched orientation between thalamic and callosal afferents, a precise correspondence of retinotopic position, as evidenced by the high degree of receptive field overlap, which increases progressively with normal binocular experience (10). This was compatible with the classic view that callosal connections perform a point-to-point topographic mapping between corresponding loci located in symmetric regions of the two hemispheres, and thereby contribute to the construction of receptive fields in the vicinity of the vertical midline.…”
Section: Functional Implications Of the Topographic Divergence Of Calmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…b) Extensive receptive field mapping of the 17/18 border, confirming earlier recordings from the fibers running within the posterior part of the corpus callosum (8), indicates that the callosally connected parts of visual areas include, on each side, the representation of the vertical midline together with a narrow portion of the ipsilateral hemifield (7). c) Recordings from split-chiasm cats, in which the geniculocortical and transcallosal pathways can be differentially activated by visual stimulation of one or the other eye, reveal that ipsi-and contralateral inputs converging onto callosal recipient neurons are closely matched in terms of retinotopic location, size, and orientation selectivity (9,10). d) Taken as a whole, these and other observations suggest that, indeed, callosal projections are functionally equivalent to corticocortical projections, being only longer-reaching (1,6).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%