1986
DOI: 10.1016/0022-0965(86)90055-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pattern perception in infants: Effects of structure and transformation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
28
0
2

Year Published

1987
1987
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
3
28
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, "good" forms have been shown to attract more attention and to be encoded more rapidly than "poor" forms in 4-montholds (Humphrey & Humphrey, 1989;Humphrey et a!., 1986;Younger & Gotlieb, 1988). Moreover, discrimination performance between pairs of"good" forms has been found to be superior to discrimination performance between pairs of "poor" forms in infants of the same age range (Humphrey et a!., 1986), a finding that is consistent with an anchor effect in the psychological space around "good" forms. However, the physical difference between the pairs of "good" forms was not equivalent to that between the pairs of "poor" forms, thus leaving open the question of whether discriminability-related reference point effects will occur when physical distances between pairs of "good" and "poor" forms are kept constant.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…In addition, "good" forms have been shown to attract more attention and to be encoded more rapidly than "poor" forms in 4-montholds (Humphrey & Humphrey, 1989;Humphrey et a!., 1986;Younger & Gotlieb, 1988). Moreover, discrimination performance between pairs of"good" forms has been found to be superior to discrimination performance between pairs of "poor" forms in infants of the same age range (Humphrey et a!., 1986), a finding that is consistent with an anchor effect in the psychological space around "good" forms. However, the physical difference between the pairs of "good" forms was not equivalent to that between the pairs of "poor" forms, thus leaving open the question of whether discriminability-related reference point effects will occur when physical distances between pairs of "good" and "poor" forms are kept constant.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…suit in greater initial looking times during the first part of the familiarization phase (Bornstein, 1978;Humphrey & Humphrey, 1989). Second, on the assumption that "good" gestalts are processed (i.e., encoded) more efficiently than "poor" gestalts, one would expect that the rate of habituation would be faster for "good" gestalts, relative to "poor" gestalts and to distortions of both "good" and "poor" gestalts (Humphrey et a\., 1986). Third, if "good" gestalts function as anchors or magnets in a domain, one would expect overall superior or inferior discrimination performance (i.e., higher or lower novelty preference scores) in the "good" gestalt discrimination task, as compared with the "poor" gestalt discrimination task (Acker et a\., 1995;Kuhl, 1991).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Van Giffen & Haith, 1984;Humphrey et al, 1986;Ghim, 1990;Quinn et al, 1993). However, to our knowledge, no prior study has examined whether infants are sensitive to a change in a single element in a pattern that comprised a good form versus one that comprised a poor form.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%