2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pavlovian to instrumental transfer: A neurobehavioural perspective

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

23
282
2
10

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 287 publications
(317 citation statements)
references
References 160 publications
23
282
2
10
Order By: Relevance
“…It describes the capacity of an appetitive pavlovian stimulus (CS+) to elicit or increase the vigor of an ongoing behavior in an instrumental conditioning procedure. Two main types of PIT have been distinguished: general and specific (Balleine and Killcross, 2006;Holmes et al, 2010). In general PIT, the CS+ enhances any appetitive instrumental response, even when the instrumental response is associated with a different outcome, whereas in specific PIT, the CS+ associated with a certain outcome only enhances an instrumental response associated with the same outcome (Fig.…”
Section: Pavlovian-to-instrumental Transfer (Pit)mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It describes the capacity of an appetitive pavlovian stimulus (CS+) to elicit or increase the vigor of an ongoing behavior in an instrumental conditioning procedure. Two main types of PIT have been distinguished: general and specific (Balleine and Killcross, 2006;Holmes et al, 2010). In general PIT, the CS+ enhances any appetitive instrumental response, even when the instrumental response is associated with a different outcome, whereas in specific PIT, the CS+ associated with a certain outcome only enhances an instrumental response associated with the same outcome (Fig.…”
Section: Pavlovian-to-instrumental Transfer (Pit)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3C). Specific PIT can be interpreted as activation of the learned behavioral sequence associated with the outcome predicted by the presented CS+ (the behavioral chaining account, Holmes et al, 2010;Van den Bos et al, 2004). General PIT can be thought of as an expression of the transfer of incentive salience and the associated energy expenditure to obtain reward, from the CS+ to the ongoing behavior (Berridge, 2007;Holmes et al, 2010), which is accompanied by a change in the motivational state (Holland and Gallagher, 2003).…”
Section: Pavlovian-to-instrumental Transfer (Pit)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is demonstrated in two types of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (PIT), general and outcome-specific PIT. In both types of PIT, appetitive CSs enhance and aversive CSs suppress instrumental behaviours for other outcomes (Estes and Skinner, 1941;Rescorla and Solomon, 1967;Lovibond, 1983;Cardinal et al, 2002;Niv et al, 2007;Holmes et al, 2010;Talmi et al, 2008;Huys et al, 2011). In general PIT, a stimulus that has been paired in a Pavlovian manner with one type of outcome (e.g.…”
Section: Pavlovian-instrumental Transfermentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This does require representation of the actual outcome, not just the value. Thus, while general PIT requires only the information carried by V MF (s), outcome-specific PIT requires additional information and likely relies on V MB (s) from model-based processes (Corbit and Balleine, 2005;Schoenbaum et al, 2009;Holmes et al, 2010;McDannald et al, 2011;Prévost et al, 2012Prévost et al, , 2013.…”
Section: Pavlovian-instrumental Transfermentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As such, cues can guide goal-directed 24 behavior, but not necessarily in an adaptive way (Holmes, Marchand, & Coutureau, 2010 a cue predicting pain increased pain-related 18 fear and avoidance tendencies, whereas cues predicting reward decreased these tendencies (Claes, Vlaeyen, 19 & Crombez, 2016). In the current experiment, we therefore focus specifically on the impact of Pavlovian 20 cues on the avoidance of painful movements.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%