1987
DOI: 10.2307/1170234
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Peer Models and Children's Behavioral Change

Abstract: This article critically reviews the research literature on peer modeling among children as a function of model attributes. Peer modeling is hypothesized to depend in part on perceived similarity between model and observer. Similarity serves as an important source of information for gauging behavioral appropriateness, formulating outcome expectations, and assessing one's self-efficacy for learning or performing tasks. Research is reviewed on the effects of model age, model sex, model competence, number of model… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

14
284
0
28

Year Published

1991
1991
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 219 publications
(326 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
(77 reference statements)
14
284
0
28
Order By: Relevance
“…These caveats regarding domain noted, we believe our results nonetheless suggest that past conclusions that similar models have more influence than dissimilar models likely have been wrong in some instances and more generally have oversimplified the influence of model similarity (e.g., Bandura, 1986;Berger, 1977;Schunk, 1987). Studies demonstrating, for example, that children more frequently imitate same-than opposite-sex models (e.g., Wolf, 1973), although typically taken as evidence that similar models produce more influence, could reflect also, or instead, that children avoid acting like opposite-sex others-that is, negative modeling.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 59%
“…These caveats regarding domain noted, we believe our results nonetheless suggest that past conclusions that similar models have more influence than dissimilar models likely have been wrong in some instances and more generally have oversimplified the influence of model similarity (e.g., Bandura, 1986;Berger, 1977;Schunk, 1987). Studies demonstrating, for example, that children more frequently imitate same-than opposite-sex models (e.g., Wolf, 1973), although typically taken as evidence that similar models produce more influence, could reflect also, or instead, that children avoid acting like opposite-sex others-that is, negative modeling.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 59%
“…The frequency and immediacy of enactive feedback also created higher perceptions of personal efficacy (Schunk, 1983). When students were taught to attribute their enactive feedback to effort, they perceived greater progress, maintained higher motivation, and reported greater efficacy for further learning (Schunk, 1987). In these investigations, Schunk and his colleagues not only demonstrated the sensitivity of efficacy beliefs to instructional interventions, but also the mediational role of these beliefs in explaining changes in learners' self-regulation and achievement outcomes (Berry, 1987;Schunk, 1981).…”
Section: Instructional and Social Influences On Self-efficacy Beliefsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Social cognitive theory [35] specifies that individuals learn behaviors by observing and imitating others, a process referred to as observational learning or modeling. This process has been extensively studied in motor skill learning and educational contexts (e.g., see [36,37]). Aside from an individual possessing the fundamental capacity to produce an observed behavior, learning a behavior is dependent on the degree to which one attends to relevant elements of a model's behavior, retains SYSTEMATIC REVIEW those elements, and is motivated to produce the action.…”
Section: Theoretical Rationale To Apply Peer Interventions To Physicamentioning
confidence: 99%