2021
DOI: 10.1080/13218719.2021.1910588
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perceptions of familiar and unfamiliar ear- and eyewitnesses

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
2
1

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While these cues are weaker and less informative than faces (Hanley & Damjanovic, 2009; Stevenage et al, 2013), speech obtained from a criminal suspect can be submitted as forensic evidence in a court of law (Edmond et al, 2011; McGorrery & McMahon, 2017; Robson, 2017). Within the context of policing and the criminal justice system, listeners are likely to be unfamiliar with the target identity, and must rely on memory (e.g., from a crime scene interaction; Harvey et al, 2021) or perceptual matching ability (i.e., deciding whether two voices can be attributed to a common identity; Mullikin & Rahman, 2010; Smith et al, 2019, 2020), to accurately identify a voice. However, research on similar processes in unfamiliar face identification have shown that such judgements are prone to error (see Young & Burton, 2018), and several studies suggest that accurately judging the identity of unfamiliar voices might be even more problematic (see Lavan, Burton, et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While these cues are weaker and less informative than faces (Hanley & Damjanovic, 2009; Stevenage et al, 2013), speech obtained from a criminal suspect can be submitted as forensic evidence in a court of law (Edmond et al, 2011; McGorrery & McMahon, 2017; Robson, 2017). Within the context of policing and the criminal justice system, listeners are likely to be unfamiliar with the target identity, and must rely on memory (e.g., from a crime scene interaction; Harvey et al, 2021) or perceptual matching ability (i.e., deciding whether two voices can be attributed to a common identity; Mullikin & Rahman, 2010; Smith et al, 2019, 2020), to accurately identify a voice. However, research on similar processes in unfamiliar face identification have shown that such judgements are prone to error (see Young & Burton, 2018), and several studies suggest that accurately judging the identity of unfamiliar voices might be even more problematic (see Lavan, Burton, et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While these cues are weaker and less informative than faces (Hanley & Danjamovic, 2009;Stevenage et al, 2013), speech obtained from a criminal suspect can be submitted as forensic evidence in a court of law (Edmond et al, 2011;McGorry & McMahon, 2017;Robson, 2017). Within the context of policing and the criminal justice system, listeners are likely to be unfamiliar with the target identity, and must rely on memory (e.g., from a crime scene interaction; Harvey et al, 2021) or perceptual matching ability (i.e., deciding whether two voices can be attributed to a common identity; Mullikin & Rahman, 2010;Smith et al, 2019), to accurately identify a voice. However, research on similar processes in unfamiliar face identification have shown that such judgements are prone to error (see , and several studies suggest that accurately judging the identity of unfamiliar voices might be just as problematic (see Lavan et al, 2019a).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%