2004
DOI: 10.3758/bf03194988
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perceptual asymmetries associated with changing-loudness aftereffects

Abstract: Listening to decreasing sound level leads to an increasing-loudness aftereffect, whereas listening to increasing sound level leads to a decreasing-loudness aftereffect. Measuring the aftereffects by nulling them in short test stimuli reveals that increasing-loudness aftereffects are greater than decreasingloudness aftereffects. However, this perceptual asymmetry may be due to another illusion-the growing-louder effect: In the absence of any adaptation, short steady stimuli are heard as growing louder. In an ex… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These surprising but consistent asymmetric results could root in the inherent properties of auditory perception. Asymmetry of loudness perception and neural responses has been reported in various auditory tasks, such as auditory habituation ( Butler, 1968 ), loudness recalibration ( Marks, 1994 ; Mapes-Riordan and Yost, 1999 ), loudness adaptation ( Canévet et al, 1985 ), and changing-loudness after effect ( Reinhardt-Rutland, 2004 ). Interestingly, the changing-loudness after effect can also be induced if participants adapted to visual changing-depth, e.g., a box expanding or shrinking ( Kitagawa and Ichihara, 2002 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These surprising but consistent asymmetric results could root in the inherent properties of auditory perception. Asymmetry of loudness perception and neural responses has been reported in various auditory tasks, such as auditory habituation ( Butler, 1968 ), loudness recalibration ( Marks, 1994 ; Mapes-Riordan and Yost, 1999 ), loudness adaptation ( Canévet et al, 1985 ), and changing-loudness after effect ( Reinhardt-Rutland, 2004 ). Interestingly, the changing-loudness after effect can also be induced if participants adapted to visual changing-depth, e.g., a box expanding or shrinking ( Kitagawa and Ichihara, 2002 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the visual world, objects approaching and receding from the observer can create ambiguous motion or be used to form ambiguous percepts of the object (approaching vs. receding ambiguity, Lewis and McBeath, 2004 ; the looming effect, Schiff et al, 1962 ; Neuhoff, 2001 ; hybrid images, Brady and Oliva, 2012 ). A similar phenomenon occurs in the auditory domain: changes in volume level can be interpreted as changes in an object’s distance, causing a sound to be interpreted as drawing nearer or farther away from the observer (the Growing-Louder Effect, Rosenblum et al, 1987 ; Middlebrooks and Green, 1991 ; Reinhardt-Rutland and Ehrenstein, 1996 ), which has been the basis for a number of perceptual illusions that affect localization (Small, 1977 ; Reinhardt-Rutland, 1996 , 2004 ; Reinhardt-Rutland and Ehrenstein, 1996 ; Malinina and Andreeva, 2013 ). Coupled with the mechanism of the front-back confusion, a change in volume may influence people’s percepts of the trajectory of an imaginary object moving through space.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Analogous considerations affect Bayesian frameworks, as Mather readily admits. On the other hand, Marr's (1982) teachings have been enduring because they promote computational models of human perceptual competences; however, a computational model need not necessarily reflect how humans actually function.…”
Section: Selective Overviewmentioning
confidence: 99%