1992
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.307
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perceptual identification of contextual attributes and infant memory retrieval.

Abstract: The contribution of specific contextual attributes to recognition of a well-learned cue was examined in four experiments with 6-month-olds. 24 h after learning to move a given mobile in a distinctive visual surround by kicking, recognition of the training cue was tested in either the original context or in one in which only a single contextual attribute was altered. Retrieval was completely disrupted by all form changes involving the deletion of angles and by a chromatic figure/ground reversal, but a discrimin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
23
0

Year Published

1995
1995
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
(93 reference statements)
5
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our conclusion that, by 6 months of age, infants process information in the context automatically and in parallel is consistent with our previous finding that contextual changes involving the deletion of an angle also do not disrupt delayed recognition at 6 months (Rovee-Collier, Schechter, et al, 1992). In that study, contextual changes from squares to either circles or stripes disrupted 24-hr retention, but changes from squares to triangles did not-despite the fact that infants could discriminate the change.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our conclusion that, by 6 months of age, infants process information in the context automatically and in parallel is consistent with our previous finding that contextual changes involving the deletion of an angle also do not disrupt delayed recognition at 6 months (Rovee-Collier, Schechter, et al, 1992). In that study, contextual changes from squares to either circles or stripes disrupted 24-hr retention, but changes from squares to triangles did not-despite the fact that infants could discriminate the change.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Six-month-olds discriminate all spatial arrangements of horizontal and vertical line segments after 24 hr when the stimuli are displayed on the focal cue (Bhatt, RoveeCollier, & Weiner, 1994), and their retention is significantly impaired by a context change 24 hr after training (Boller & Rovee-Collier, 1992;Rovee-Collier, Schechter, Shyi, & Shields, 1992;. Given the results of Experiment 1, we predicted that 6-month-olds would exhibit a retention deficit if trained in a context displaying these same stimulus configurations and tested in the presence of a different configuration.…”
Section: Experiments 2: Delayed Recognition At 6 Monthsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Not until the hippocampus presumably attained funcminutes at most-for the better part of the first year of life (for review, see Werner & Perlmutter, tional maturity, late in the first year, was the capacity to form relatively enduring memories thought possible (Kagan & Hamburg, 1981;Schacter & Moscovitch, 1984). Using an operant learning par-however, our laboratory has documented longto 12-month-olds, using portable but larger computer-driven trains in highly complex displays term retention on a different order of magnitude, ranging from 2 days at 2 months of age (Vander (Klein, Gilch, Bhatt, Hartshorn, & Rovee-Collier, 1994;Klein et al, 1993). However, the duration Linde, Morrongiello, & Rovee-Collier, 1985) to 2 weeks at 6 months of age (Hill, Borovsky, & of retention of these older infants was considerably longer than we had anticipated on the basis of .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, we wondered if memory reactivation could be demonstrated in the train task and, after earlier work of Butler (1954) and Fagen and Ohr (1986), for use with 9-to 12-month-olds if so, whether some of the major variables that affect reactivation and the subsequent expression (Klein, Wondoloski, Carvalho, Wurtzel, & Bhatt, 1993). Butler had taught monkeys to open a winof the reactivated memory in the mobile task (e.g., the specificity of the training cue and training condow by lever pressing in order to view a train moving around a track on a table-top in an adjatext as retrieval cues) would similarly affect them in the train task.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The study of early memory development has come a long way from its initial concerns with whether and how early infants learn, to its current focus on how long infants can remember what they learned and under what conditions (Rovee-Collier, 1996). Researchers have found that infant memory is influenced by several factors, including amount and distribution of training (Ohr, Fagen, Rovee-Collier, Hayne, & Linde, 1989), the match between the focal object of learning and contextual cues present at acquisition and recall (Rovee-Collier, Schechter, Shyi, & Shields, 1992), the amount of information in terms of number of components in a sequence (Bauer, 1995), postevent information (if novel or familiar) along with its location (whether central or peripheral to the target) and timing (Boller, Grabelle, & Rovee-Collier, 1995), the organization of information to be retained (i.e., when ordered causally or arbitrarily, see Bauer, 1997), and reminder techniques such as reactivation and reinstatement (Rovee-Collier, 1995), to name a few. While these findings from memory research have informed us about a number of mechanisms involved in learning and memory retrieval during early development, less attention has been paid to understanding infant memory by manipulating the perceptual experience of young organisms.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%