2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.04.012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Performance predictions affect attentional processes of event-based prospective memory

Abstract: Abstract:To investigate whether making performance predictions affects prospective memory (PM) processing, we asked one group of participants to predict their performance in a PM task embedded in an ongoing task and compared their performance with a control group that made no predictions. A third group gave not only PM predictions but also ongoing-task predictions. Exclusive PM predictions resulted in slower ongoing-task responding both in a nonfocal (Experiment 1) and in a focal (Experiment 2) PM task. Only i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
20
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
3
20
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Consistent with earlier studies of prospective memory tasks (Gilbert, 2015b;Meeks et al, 2007;Rummel et al, 2013;Schnitzspahn, Zeintl, et al, 2011), participants were underconfident in their self-judgements of accuracy using internal memory (advised group: t(54) = 3.6, p < .001, d = .98; unadvised group: t(52) = 4.3, p < .0001, d = 1.2).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 86%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Consistent with earlier studies of prospective memory tasks (Gilbert, 2015b;Meeks et al, 2007;Rummel et al, 2013;Schnitzspahn, Zeintl, et al, 2011), participants were underconfident in their self-judgements of accuracy using internal memory (advised group: t(54) = 3.6, p < .001, d = .98; unadvised group: t(52) = 4.3, p < .0001, d = 1.2).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 86%
“…In the present study, participants were underconfident about their internal memory abilities (apart from the Easy, Positive group in Experiment 3) and over-used reminders. Underconfidence is the pattern generally found in laboratory studies of prospective memory (Gilbert, 2015b;Meeks et al, 2007;Rummel, Kuhlmann, & Touron, 2013;. However, in other situations individuals' metacognitive judgements (and hence cost/benefit decisions) may be biased in the opposite direction, potentially including naturalistic prospective memory tasks executed in everyday life.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The three main dimensions of executive functions (inhibition, shifting and updating; see Miyake et al, 2000) seems to be linked to PM performance (Schnitzspahn, Stahl, Zeintl, Kaller, & Kliegel, 2013). It is also applies to binding in working memory (Blondelle et al, 2015; Gonneaud et al, 2011; Hainselin et al, 2011), working memory (Cherry & LeCompte, 1999; West & Craik, 2001), processing speed (West & Craik, 2001; Zeintl, Kliegel, & Hofer, 2007), and metamemory (Meeks, Hicks, & Marsh, 2007; Meier, von Wartburg, Matter, Rothen, & Reber, 2011; Rummel, Kuhlmann, & Touron, 2013; Schnitzspahn, Zeintl, Jäger, & Kliegel, 2011; Smith, Souchay, & Moulin, 2011). …”
Section: Pm: Cognitive Processes and Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, one has to consider a potential complication when assessing item-level JOLs for laboratory and naturalistic PM tasks that may be particularly relevant for PM research. In detail, in PM tasks, performance predictions could alter PM performance itself as they may enhance PM performance by repeatedly highlighting the intention after initial encoding for future retrieval (see, e.g., Meier, Wartburg, Matter, Rothen, & Reber, 2011 who showed that performance predictions may make the intention more accessible or Rummel, Kuhlmann & Touron, 2013, who found that PM performance increased in a prediction group compared to a control group without performance predictions).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%