2016
DOI: 10.1044/2015_jslhr-h-14-0173
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Persistent Language Delay Versus Late Language Emergence in Children With Early Cochlear Implantation

Abstract: Purpose: The purpose of the present investigation is to differentiate children using cochlear implants (CIs) who did or did not achieve age-appropriate language scores by midelementary grades and to identify risk factors for persistent language delay following early cochlear implantation. Materials and Method: Children receiving unilateral CIs at young ages (12-38 months) were tested longitudinally and classified with normal language emergence (n = 19), late language emergence (n = 22), or persistent language … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
92
0
5

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 113 publications
(103 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
6
92
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…To be included in the present study, children had to (a) receive cochlear implantation before age 30 months and have used the Nucleus CI 24R or Nucleus CI 24M devices (Cochlear Ltd., Sydney, Australia) with the processing strategy of the advanced combination encoder (ACE; Cochlear Ltd., 2010; excluding 39 children) and (b) participate in the story-retell task in the annual evaluation 3, 4, and 5 years after implantation and produce at least five utterances with verbs for analysis at each time point (Lahey, Liebergott, Chesnick, Menyuk, & Adams, 1992; excluding 20 children). We included children who received cochlear implantation before age 30 months in order to reduce the effect of age of implantation (Geers et al, 2016). Although the current population of pediatric CI users typically receives cochlear implantation before age 24 months, Nicholas and Geers (2007) indicate that children who received implantation between ages 12 and 30 months show a similar growth rate in language development, which supports the use of 30 months as the cutoff for age of implantation in the present study.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 48%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…To be included in the present study, children had to (a) receive cochlear implantation before age 30 months and have used the Nucleus CI 24R or Nucleus CI 24M devices (Cochlear Ltd., Sydney, Australia) with the processing strategy of the advanced combination encoder (ACE; Cochlear Ltd., 2010; excluding 39 children) and (b) participate in the story-retell task in the annual evaluation 3, 4, and 5 years after implantation and produce at least five utterances with verbs for analysis at each time point (Lahey, Liebergott, Chesnick, Menyuk, & Adams, 1992; excluding 20 children). We included children who received cochlear implantation before age 30 months in order to reduce the effect of age of implantation (Geers et al, 2016). Although the current population of pediatric CI users typically receives cochlear implantation before age 24 months, Nicholas and Geers (2007) indicate that children who received implantation between ages 12 and 30 months show a similar growth rate in language development, which supports the use of 30 months as the cutoff for age of implantation in the present study.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 48%
“…We want to point out that children with CIs in the present study all used the ACE processing strategy, which is still the default processing strategy for the Nucleus CI devices (Cochlear Ltd., 2010;Wolfe & Schafer, 2010). Although variation of CI technology may influence speech-language outcomes (Geers et al, 2016), to date there is no published evidence suggesting that different CI processors (e.g., SPRINT, CP910 in cochlear devices) with the same processing strategy (e.g., ACE) would result in significant differences in speech-language outcomes. Similarly, there is no published evidence indicating that different processing strategies from different makes in the same time period would lead to significantly different outcomes.…”
Section: Limitations and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…32 The relatively high proportion of children in the no sign language exposure group achieving scores within 1 SD of normal hearing agemates replicated results observed previously for a nationwide sample of 60 children who had no early sign language exposure, received a CI within the same age range (ie, by 38 months), and were approximately the same age at assessment near early and late elementary grades. 33 The percentage of children exhibiting a language delay that persisted through elementary grades was strikingly similar in these 2 studies (29% and 32%), indicating generalizability of this result.…”
Section: Does Early Exposure To Sign Language In Addition To Speech Psupporting
confidence: 50%
“…Similar patterns are evident wherever the full distribution of spoken language outcomes is reported (e.g. Ching et al, 2013;Geers et al, 2016;Niparko et al, 2010). Meanwhile, the likelihood of developing proficiency in ASL among DHH children who have partial access to ASL is virtually unknown, due in part to the fact that few studies distinguish natural sign languages from other forms of manual communication, and that even those few that make this distinction do not assess proficiency in the relevant sign language (e.g.…”
mentioning
confidence: 49%