2012
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2060
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Person (mis)perception: functionally biased sex categorization of bodies

Abstract: Social perception is among the most important tasks that occur in daily life, and perceivers readily appreciate the social affordances of others. Here, we demonstrate that sex categorizations are functionally biased towards a male percept. Perceivers judged body shapes that varied in waist-to-hip ratio to be men if they were not, in reality, exclusive to women, and male categorizations occurred more quickly than female categorizations (studies 1 and 4). This pattern was corroborated when participants identifie… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
71
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(76 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
5
71
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In Study 3, our results seem consistent with previous research and revealed an influence of morphology on categorization (Johnson et al 2012;Johnson and Tassinary 2005). Following our hypothesis, the prime did influence categorization in the direction of stereotypes when perceptual cues were ambiguous.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In Study 3, our results seem consistent with previous research and revealed an influence of morphology on categorization (Johnson et al 2012;Johnson and Tassinary 2005). Following our hypothesis, the prime did influence categorization in the direction of stereotypes when perceptual cues were ambiguous.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 93%
“…In the case of gender, the Waist-to-Hip Ratio (WHR) of a target seems to be particularly relevant to categorize individuals by gender-with hourglass figures being categorized as female and tubular figures as male (Johnson et al 2012;Johnson and Tassinary 2005). Regarding top-down influences, cues that trigger gender stereotypes may also influence the gendercategorization of a target.…”
Section: The Present Studiesmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The BMI cutoff for overweight is 25, but women consider themselves overweight at approximately 23, which is well within the range for normal weight (Crawford & Campbell, 1999). Similar effects emerge among observers, who believe that the average female body is actually a thin shape that does not exist in nature (Johnson, Iida, & Tassinary, 2012). As evidenced by the study by Major and colleagues (2014), the perception of oneself as overweight is important when considering the consequences of weight stigma, and individuals who are not objectively overweight or obese may still fall prey to stigma processes.…”
Section: Entering the Cyclesupporting
confidence: 49%
“…Bülthoff, 2012;Cellerino, Borghetti, & Sartucci, 2004) that has also been reported in chimpanzees (de Waal & Pokorny, 2008). A similar 'male bias' has also been found for body shape (Johnson, Iida, & Tassinary, 2012), hands (Gaetano, van der Zwan, Blair, & Brooks, 2014) and biological motion (Troje, Sadr, Geyer, & Nakayama, 2006). It has been suggested that, in the history of humans, misclassifying a man as female has generally proved to be potentially more dangerous than misclassifying a woman as male (Armann & Bülthoff, 2012).…”
Section: Bias In the Perception Of A Person's Sexmentioning
confidence: 69%