2009
DOI: 10.1007/s00422-009-0347-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phase-linking and the perceived motion during off-vertical axis rotation

Abstract: Human off-vertical axis rotation (OVAR) in the dark typically produces perceived motion about a cone, the amplitude of which changes as a function of frequency. This perception is commonly attributed to the fact that both the OVAR and the conical motion have a gravity vector that rotates about the subject. Little-known, however, is that this rotating-gravity explanation for perceived conical motion is inconsistent with basic observations about self-motion perception: (a) that the perceived vertical moves towar… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
16
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 106 publications
(171 reference statements)
0
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…the percent of the 16 possible (4×4) in which a stimulus (head angular, head linear, body angular, body linear) was in the roll direction of cross-coupling in a yaw movement (NUP-to-RED, RED-to-NUP, NUP-to-LED, LED-to-NUP). For linear stimuli, rightward and leftward linear velocities were considered as being in the same direction as rightward and leftward roll, respectively, and similarly for forward and backward linear velocities and pitch, based upon evidence of linear-angular perceptual alignment [13]. …”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…the percent of the 16 possible (4×4) in which a stimulus (head angular, head linear, body angular, body linear) was in the roll direction of cross-coupling in a yaw movement (NUP-to-RED, RED-to-NUP, NUP-to-LED, LED-to-NUP). For linear stimuli, rightward and leftward linear velocities were considered as being in the same direction as rightward and leftward roll, respectively, and similarly for forward and backward linear velocities and pitch, based upon evidence of linear-angular perceptual alignment [13]. …”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…8], a nonlinearity that holds regardless of whether the angular velocities are of the centrifuge [8] or the head (confirmed though follow-up testing). In addition, perceptions of angular and linear motion affect each other in centrifuges [10, 12] and during off-vertical axis rotation [13]. In other words, there is evidence that a subject’s perception of angular motion may be affected by the linear stimulus, and vice versa.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is important, since in the absence of vision, perception of vertical is altered by the changing gravito-inertial vector that results from any acceleration not aligned with the constant gravitational background force (i.e., the hilltop illusion) [14], [18], [19], [47]. In this study, no visual tilt is present in the visual input other than the visual tilt directly resulting from self-generated head tilts.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…A switch from visual to vestibular dominance over control of the head is likely related to the slow response times of retinal slip detection involved in the optokinetic reflex (OKR) and the much faster vestibular-ocular reflex (VOR). The cutoff frequency of these sensory systems transitioning from visual to vestibular dominance has been reported to be in the bandwidth of 0.2-0.5 Hz [2], [19], [24], [34]. Another driving factor that may affect how important visual versus inertial inputs are for head stabilization relates to the visual system's ability to remain sensitive to constant velocity cues, while the vestibular system relies on velocity change, i.e., acceleration [5].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The equations for the model are analogous to those previously developed for sinusoidal motion [27, 28]. The variables:…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%