1931
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1931.tb00597.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phenomenal Regression to the Real Object. I

Abstract: I. The apparent shapes of Jisures observed obliquely (pp. 339-347). 11. Hering's observations on ' GwZch!niss$arben~ (pp. 347-351). Experimentson apparent sizes (pp. 351-355). IV. The tendency to equalize wrtical semi-axes i n perception of tibe inclined square and the phenomenal regression of parallel lines (pp. 355-358). V. Summary (pp. 358-359). 1 H. J. M'att, for example, a y e : ''Generally 8 plate looks what it 'really' is, circular, not elliptical as the retinal image of it really is(l)." J. of Psych. X… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
122
2
1

Year Published

1946
1946
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 167 publications
(134 citation statements)
references
References 1 publication
9
122
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The results of Experiment 1 replicate the findings of experiments demonstrating the effects of shape constancy (e.g., Thouless, 1931aThouless, , 1931b. Participants consistently chose an outline representing a more "rectangular" view of the window than the view in the photographs.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 78%
“…The results of Experiment 1 replicate the findings of experiments demonstrating the effects of shape constancy (e.g., Thouless, 1931aThouless, , 1931b. Participants consistently chose an outline representing a more "rectangular" view of the window than the view in the photographs.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 78%
“…This is consistent with work that shows that observers do not process visual angles precisely and tend to enlarge smaller visual angles (Higashiyama, 1992). Much work has also shown that past history and significance, familiar size, and other cognitive processes may "add to" the visual angle or our phenomenal impressions of object size, in order to increase object size toward the actual size (Foley et al, 2004;Gilinsky, 1951;Gogel, 1974Gogel, , 1998Gogel & Da Silva, 1987;Higashiyama & Kitano, 1991;Matsushima et al, 2005;Thouless, 1931). Although verbal estimates were far longer than the visual angle predictions, it seems that the observers were utilizing some characteristics of visual angle, because the estimates of length doubled with a halving of the distance to the lines.…”
Section: Conditionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ability to perceive length and spatial extent does not necessarily improve when observers view real objects. For example, Baird and Biersdorf (1967) found that judged extents in depth of cardboard strips placed flat on a table top decreased by 21% as the strips were moved farther from the observers, from the front to the rear of the table (see also Thouless, 1931). Frisby,Buckley,and Duke (1996) recently conducted an interesting study oflength discriminations, using real "gnarled" sticks, and found that when the two sticks had different orientations and different distances (Experiment 6), the Weber fraction for binocular viewing averaged 10.6%.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous research on real-world stimuli has used gnarled sticks (Frisby et aI., 1996) or relatively flat ground planes or flat table tops (Baird & Biersdorf, 1967;Gilinsky, 1951;Harway, 1963;Loomis et aI., 1992;Thouless, 1931;Wagner, 1985). It is important to keep in mind, however, that most of the time, human observers are perceiving 3-D objects placed against backgrounds.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%