2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2010.08.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phonetic reduction and informational redundancy in self-initiated self-repair in Dutch

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
16
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

3
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
9
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In relation to EXPECTATION 5, our analysis confirms Plug's (2011) finding of a predominance of temporal compression that is, a local increase in speech tempo following the reparandum offset. As indicated above, given that EXPECTATION 1 is not met, we cannot explain this predominance in general probabilistic terms: while a predominant fall in informativeness between reparandum and repair items is theoretically plausible, none of our probabilistic measures suggest that this is present in our dataset.…”
Section:  Expectationsupporting
confidence: 82%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In relation to EXPECTATION 5, our analysis confirms Plug's (2011) finding of a predominance of temporal compression that is, a local increase in speech tempo following the reparandum offset. As indicated above, given that EXPECTATION 1 is not met, we cannot explain this predominance in general probabilistic terms: while a predominant fall in informativeness between reparandum and repair items is theoretically plausible, none of our probabilistic measures suggest that this is present in our dataset.…”
Section:  Expectationsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…So far, the only examination of the relationship between informativeness and repair tempo is that of Plug (2011). Consistent with the first prediction above, Plug (2011) reports a predominance of temporal compression that is, a relative speeding up after the repair initiation in a collection of Dutch self-repairs.…”
Section: Informativeness and Articulatory Reductionmentioning
confidence: 75%
“…Fromkin 1973, Baars et al 1975, Nooteboom 1980, Levelt 1983, Levelt & Cutler 1983, Blackmer & Mitton 1991, Postma & Kolk 1993, Shattuck-Hufnagel & Cutler 1999, Shriberg 2001, Jasperson 2002, Nooteboom 2005a, Hartsuiker 2006, Seyfeddinipur et al 2008, Nooteboom 2010, Plug 2011, Tydgat et al 2011. For phoneticians, the interest lies primarily in how speakers deal with the disfluency associated with repair, while for psycholinguists repair 'may reveal principles of organization of the speech production process that would be hard to discover on the basis of laboratory data alone' (Levelt 1984: 105) -in particular with reference to self-monitoring.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Shriberg (2001) suggests that this prolongation is different from typical phrase-and sentence-final lengthening and that its intonation also differs, in that it does not display the boundary tones that are characteristic of phrase and sentence boundaries. Shriberg (2001), Nakatani and Hirschberg (1994) and Plug (2011) give descriptions of the acoustic-phonetic properties of reparanda, the editing phase and repairs, which will not be detailed here. It is worth mentioning that, although there are acoustic features in reparanda that can be detected on close inspection, they may not always be perceptually salient: Listeners are not often able to detect that a repair is about to occur, when they have heard the full reparandum .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…), The handbook of speech production (pp. 445-469) Wiley-Blackwell 43 aspects of repair (Nakatani and Hirschberg 1994, Plug 2011, Shriberg 2001. We focus on one prominent aspect.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%