2019
DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2019.1577138
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phonological Motivation for the Acquisition of Onomatopoeia: An Analysis of Early Words

Abstract: Onomatopoeia are disproportionately high in number in infants' early words compared to adult language. Studies of infant language perception have proposed an iconic advantage for onomatopoeia, which may make them easier for infants to learn (Imai & Kita, 2014;Laing, 2017). This study analyses infants' early word production to show a phonological motivation for onomatopoeia in early acquisition. Cross-linguistic evidence from 16 infants demonstrates how these forms fit within a phonologically-systematic develop… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Another issue to consider is the existence of alternative onomatopoeic verbal labels for some of the sounds (e.g., "bowwow", "moo"), frequently used in childdirected speech and thus acquired early in live. Given that age of acquisition is a powerful variable affecting lexical processing (see Laing, 2019, for a review), it could be that these alternative verbal labels, even though not used frequently in adult language, were strongly activated by sounds specifically and then acted as powerful competitors, causing the boost in interference. According to our intuition, there are 4 items (out of the 16 used in Experiments 1 and 2) for which such onomatopoeic verbal labels are often used in child language (dog: "bowwow"; cow: "moo"; pig: "oink", motorcycle: "vroom").…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another issue to consider is the existence of alternative onomatopoeic verbal labels for some of the sounds (e.g., "bowwow", "moo"), frequently used in childdirected speech and thus acquired early in live. Given that age of acquisition is a powerful variable affecting lexical processing (see Laing, 2019, for a review), it could be that these alternative verbal labels, even though not used frequently in adult language, were strongly activated by sounds specifically and then acted as powerful competitors, causing the boost in interference. According to our intuition, there are 4 items (out of the 16 used in Experiments 1 and 2) for which such onomatopoeic verbal labels are often used in child language (dog: "bowwow"; cow: "moo"; pig: "oink", motorcycle: "vroom").…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As young infants acquire language within culture, they are likely to first embody words whose iconicity is universally perceivable because the sound‐meaning correspondences of these words are easy for them to recognize, and adult speakers adjust their input as well (Laing, 2019); but they gradually deviate from such primary iconicity and acquire the abstract language system possessed by adults. Through this process, they develop secondary iconicity and embody language‐specific systems.…”
Section: How Does Language‐specific Iconicity Arise and Get Embodied?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In spoken languages, iconicity includes onomatopoeia, i.e., words representing sounds of animals, vehicles, or other objects, as well as sound symbolism, i.e., phonemes or words that match properties of their referents such as their shape or movement ( Ota et al, 2018 ). Onomatopoeic words are more prevalent in the vocabulary of young children as well as in child-directed speech and are discussed to facilitate vocabulary acquisition ( Ota et al, 2018 ; Laing, 2019 ; Motamedi et al, 2021 ). Accordingly, sound symbolism is already perceived and exploited by babies ( Peña et al, 2011 ; Ozturk et al, 2013 ) and continues to be involved in vocabulary learning by children of different ages ( Maurer et al, 2006 ; Imai et al, 2008 ; Massaro and Perlman, 2017 ; Tzeng et al, 2017 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%