2019
DOI: 10.5334/gjgl.826
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phonotactic restrictions and morphology in Aymara

Abstract: Nonlocal phonological interactions are often sensitive to morphological domains. Bolivian Aymara restricts the cooccurrence of plain, ejective, and aspirated stops within, but not across, morphemes. We document these restrictions in a morphologically parsed corpus of Aymara. We further present two experiments with native Aymara speakers. In the first experiment, speakers are asked to repeat nonce words that should be interpreted as monomorphemic. Speakers are more accurate at repeating nonce words that respect… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
11
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
1
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The constraint *+TT should be weighted highly enough to capture the clear OCP effect in W B while *T+T should be weighted lowly enough to allow two marked consonants to still co-occur over a word boundary. The OCP pattern of Korean compound tensification corroborates the observation that some phonological restrictions that hold within a morpheme can be loosened or even lifted at morpheme boundaries (Martin 2011;Gallagher et al 2019).…”
Section: Analysis: Word Boundary and Counting Cumulativitysupporting
confidence: 78%
“…The constraint *+TT should be weighted highly enough to capture the clear OCP effect in W B while *T+T should be weighted lowly enough to allow two marked consonants to still co-occur over a word boundary. The OCP pattern of Korean compound tensification corroborates the observation that some phonological restrictions that hold within a morpheme can be loosened or even lifted at morpheme boundaries (Martin 2011;Gallagher et al 2019).…”
Section: Analysis: Word Boundary and Counting Cumulativitysupporting
confidence: 78%
“…Speakers, however, may have a strong harmony preference (or not), which is presumably learned from a morphologically informed analysis of the data. In languages where harmony holds only of roots or stems, a phonotactic learner may be more successful if morpheme boundaries are represented in the learning data (e.g., the simulation of a root-bound laryngeal restriction in Aymara in Gallagher et al 2019). Morpheme-specific alternations require a model of morphophonological learning (e.g., Albright & Hayes (2003) Gouskova & Becker (2013), Allen & Becker (2015)), that may represent knowledge of alternations as distinct from phonotactics.…”
Section: General Discussion and Conclusionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Inductive Projection Learner (Gouskova & Gallagher 2020;Gallagher et al 2019) builds on the UCLA Phonotactic Learner (Hayes & Wilson 2008), implementing an inductive procedure for postulating nonlocal projections. 1 The UCLA Phonotactic Learner takes as input a list of attested word forms in a language (the training data) and a feature set that uniquely defines each segment in the list.…”
Section: Description Of the Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…MacEachern 1997). For example, in Bolivian Aymara (data from MacEachern 1997 andGallagher, Gouskova, &Camacho Rios 2019), nonidentical ejectives cannot cooccur within a morpheme (4), but they can cooccur across morpheme boundaries within the same word (5).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%