2021
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.682132
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phylogenetic Typology

Abstract: In this article we propose a novel method to estimate the frequency distribution of linguistic variables while controlling for statistical non-independence due to shared ancestry. Unlike previous approaches, our technique uses all available data, from language families large and small as well as from isolates, while controlling for different degrees of relatedness on a continuous scale estimated from the data. Our approach involves three steps: First, distributions of phylogenies are inferred from lexical data… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
25
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
0
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thirdly, in quantitative diachronic typology, it is necessary to work with cross-family data. Hence, typologists have employed unified approaches using the same setup for different families, or even hierarchical models connected by shared hyperpriors [54,55]. However, for lexical data, individual language families are often studied by separate groups of researchers with different model details and there is little indication that models are re-used and compared between different language families; this article seems to be the first such study.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thirdly, in quantitative diachronic typology, it is necessary to work with cross-family data. Hence, typologists have employed unified approaches using the same setup for different families, or even hierarchical models connected by shared hyperpriors [54,55]. However, for lexical data, individual language families are often studied by separate groups of researchers with different model details and there is little indication that models are re-used and compared between different language families; this article seems to be the first such study.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since Greenberg's seminal work, a number of studies have provided empirical evidence for crosscategory harmony (e.g., Hawkins, 1983;Dryer, 1992;Jäger and Wahle, 2021). For example, Dryer (1992) investigated whether the tendency of cross-category harmony observed in Greenberg (1963) holds for a larger sample of more than 600 languages.…”
Section: Typological Evidence For Cross-category Harmonymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, Dryer's (1992) method for controlling relationships among languages remains potentially problematic. For example, the results might be biased by the assumptions of the genetic relations of languages in the sample (see Dunn et al, 2011, Ladd, Roberts, and Dediu, 2015, Jäger and Wahle, 2021. To mitigate the effects of data independence in language samples, Bayesian phylogenetic methods have recently been used to re-evaluate the word order correlations observed in typology.…”
Section: Typological Evidence For Cross-category Harmonymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It remains an open question in which ways a similarity-based structure bias operates over time and space in the transmission and emergence of L and X unnatural patterns crosslinguistically. To probe this further, phylogenetic modelling is needed that compares the transition preferences across patterns in a wide range of language families and under different conditions of language contact (Bickel, 2015;Cathcart, 2018;Cysouw, 2011;Greenhill, Heggarty, & Gray, 2020;Jäger & Wahle, 2021).…”
Section: Diachronic Change In Unnatural Patternsmentioning
confidence: 99%