2012
DOI: 10.2190/ic.31.4.c
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Pictorial and Graphemic Processing in Fluent Aphasia

Abstract: Some research suggests that people with aphasia may have deficits in processing pictorial materials; other studies suggest this is contradictory. Moreover, it is not known how these potential deficits compare to deficits in graphemic processing. The purpose of this study was to examine pictorial processing in individuals with fluent aphasia, and how it compares to their graphemic processing. Three people with fluent aphasia were administered three computer based expectation tasks. Participants were required to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Also, the choice of sequence endings was worse for right than left hemisphere damaged patients (Bihrle, Brownell, Powelson, & Gardner, 1986), but they differed in their errors: right hemisphere damaged patients chose structurally well-formed but incoherent endings, while left hemisphere damaged patients chose coherent but less structurally intact endings. Finally, recognition of congruity for a sequence-ending image was less accurate and took longer for a Wernicke's aphasic than patients with conduction aphasia or anomia (Stead, Savage, & Buckingham, 2012).…”
Section: Aphasiamentioning
confidence: 93%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Also, the choice of sequence endings was worse for right than left hemisphere damaged patients (Bihrle, Brownell, Powelson, & Gardner, 1986), but they differed in their errors: right hemisphere damaged patients chose structurally well-formed but incoherent endings, while left hemisphere damaged patients chose coherent but less structurally intact endings. Finally, recognition of congruity for a sequence-ending image was less accurate and took longer for a Wernicke's aphasic than patients with conduction aphasia or anomia (Stead, Savage, & Buckingham, 2012).…”
Section: Aphasiamentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Comics or instruction manuals are generally assumed to be simple and universally comprehended, underlying their use in intelligence testing and clinical assessments (Kaufman & Lichtenberger, 2006;Wechsler, 1981), and as stimuli in a wide range of anthropological and experimental research. A presumed accessibility has placed visual narratives in practical contexts like instructions (Martin & Smith-Jackson, 2008;Spinillo & Dyson, 2001) and humanitarian aid materials (Fussell & Haaland, 1978;Stenchly, Feldt, Weiss, Andriamparany, & Buerkert, 2019), and has underscored efforts advocating for using comics in education (Cary, 2004;Sousanis, 2015) and science and health communication (Farinella, 2018;M. J.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They compared aphasic patients (both fluent and non‐fluent) to patients with right‐hemisphere damage and found a double dissociation such that the aphasic patients made more errors on the linguistic version, whereas the right‐hemisphere patients made more errors on the picture version. Stead, Savage, and Buckingham () found that patients with fluent aphasias performed better on a sequence completion task (participants determined whether the final stimulus in a sequence was congruent or incongruent with the preceding items) for sequences of pictures compared to sequences of words. Therefore, both of these studies demonstrate that for these groups of aphasic patients, narrative comprehension was easier when stories were presented visually compared to linguistically, which supports Prediction #2.…”
Section: Tests Of the Visual Ease Assumptionmentioning
confidence: 99%