2015
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000032
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Piloting and path integration within and across boundaries.

Abstract: Three experiments investigated whether navigation is less efficient across boundaries than within boundaries. In an immersive virtual environment, participants learned objects' locations in a large room or a small room. Participants then pointed to the objects' original locations after physically walking a circuitous path without vision. For participants who learned the objects in the large room, the testing position and the learning position were in the same room so that participants did not cross boundaries … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

4
27
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
4
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This supports views that environments are represented by a manifold of spatial maps (Derdikman & Moser, 2010; Han & Becker, 2014; Mou & Wang, 2015) rather than one unitary spatial map. In the case of unitary spatial maps, simulation speeds should not vary with spatial coherence and vividness, as the same representation is being used to make predictive inferences in all cases.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 72%
“…This supports views that environments are represented by a manifold of spatial maps (Derdikman & Moser, 2010; Han & Becker, 2014; Mou & Wang, 2015) rather than one unitary spatial map. In the case of unitary spatial maps, simulation speeds should not vary with spatial coherence and vividness, as the same representation is being used to make predictive inferences in all cases.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 72%
“…While we did not collect eye-tracking data to verify exactly where participants fixated, facing angle during navigation provided some insight into what aspects of the spatial layout participants attended to (Ekstrom et al, 2003). Together, our findings support ideas from the navigation and attentional literature that representations are updated during navigation (Burgess, 2006;Mou & Wang, 2015;Wang & Spelke, 2000) and that "top-down" and "bottom-up" mechanisms during navigation are integrated versus dichotomized (Awh et al, 2012), respectively.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 62%
“…A second reason people easily get lost in complex indoor spaces arises from research on reference frames. A large body of evidence (Diwadkar & McNamara, 1997;McNamara, Rump, & Werner, 2003;Meilinger, Riecke, & Bülthoff, 2014;Meilinger, Frankenstein, Watanabe, Bülthoff, & Hölscher, 2015;Mou & McNamara, 2002;Mou, McNamara, & Zhang, 2013;Mou & Wang, 2015;Roskos-Ewoldsen, McNamara, Shelton, & Carr, 1998;Shelton & McNamara, 2004) supports the idea that spatial knowledge is stored in a preferred reference frame. A reference frame is a spatial representation in which objects or other representations of space are contained, or with respect to which they are ordered, oriented, located, or thought to move; a preferred reference frame is an orientation in which a spatial layout is most easily recalled.…”
mentioning
confidence: 84%