2018
DOI: 10.5334/gjgl.478
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Possessors move through the edge, too

Abstract: This paper discusses possessor sub-extraction in Indonesian, Javanese and Madurese, and its implications for phase-based A-bar extraction of nominals. I show that possessors may extract from their possessive DPs and occur at the left edge of the clause. I argue that the suffix that occurs on the possessum (Indonesian-nya, Javanese-ne, Madurese-Nah) is the pronunciation of the functional head D rather than a pronominal possessor or resumptive pronoun. While the extraction of verbal arguments has been well studi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This has prompted some scholars to liken the behaviour of the pivot in Philippine languages to object shift in Germanic (Richards 2000;Rackowski 2002;Rackowski & Richards 2005;Sabbagh 2016), where the pivot obligatorily moves to the outer edge of VoiceP and becomes the highest argument visible to C for overt or covert extraction. Common to both the object shift approach and the proposal sketched here is that it is essentially Voice which determines which argument is the pivot (see also Aldridge 2004;Jeoung 2018). In my approach, the pivot is identified by virtue of having undergone nominative Case assignment by Voice.…”
Section: Tagalog Voice Morphologymentioning
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This has prompted some scholars to liken the behaviour of the pivot in Philippine languages to object shift in Germanic (Richards 2000;Rackowski 2002;Rackowski & Richards 2005;Sabbagh 2016), where the pivot obligatorily moves to the outer edge of VoiceP and becomes the highest argument visible to C for overt or covert extraction. Common to both the object shift approach and the proposal sketched here is that it is essentially Voice which determines which argument is the pivot (see also Aldridge 2004;Jeoung 2018). In my approach, the pivot is identified by virtue of having undergone nominative Case assignment by Voice.…”
Section: Tagalog Voice Morphologymentioning
confidence: 92%
“…In Section 3.3, I present the argument structure restrictions on external possession, showing that it is limited to themes of externallycaused change of state verbs. This theme restriction on external possession is found in closely related Cebuano (Bell 1976;1983) but is absent in other Austronesian languages, such Malagasy (Keenan 1972;Keenan & Ralalaoherivony 2001), Chamorro (Chung 2008) and Indonesian (Davies 2008;Jeoung 2018). The data presented in this section comes from elicitation work with four native speaker consultants.…”
Section: External Possessionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…The Indonesian sentence in (21a) is ambiguous – it can be analyzed as either a cleft sentence or a relative clause, even though they have the exactly the same structure (Jeoung 2018 and Aldridge 2008). When the Rumah Adi ‘Adi’s house’ functions as an external head of the relative clause, the relative CP does not convey any particular information structure.…”
Section: An Analysis Of Specificational Pseudocleftsmentioning
confidence: 99%